Scolaris Content Display Scolaris Content Display

Medicamentos colinérgicos para la discinesia tardía inducida por neurolépticos

Contraer todo Desplegar todo

Resumen

Antecedentes

La discinesia tardía (DT) sigue siendo un efecto adverso problemático de la medicación antipsicótica convencional (neuroléptica). Se ha propuesto que la DT podría tener un componente de deficiencia colinérgica central. Se han utilizado fármacos colinérgicos para tratar la DT.

Objetivos

Determinar los efectos de los fármacos colinérgicos (arecolina, colina, deanol, lecitina, meclofenoxato, fisostigmina, RS 86, tacrina, metoxitacrina, galantamina, ipidacrina, donepezilo, rivastigmina, eptastigmina, metrifonato, xanomelina, cevimelina) para el tratamiento de la DT inducida por antipsicóticos en personas con esquizofrenia u otra enfermedad mental crónica.

Métodos de búsqueda

Se realizó una búsqueda electrónica en el Registro Especializado de Ensayos Controlados del Grupo Cochrane de Esquizofrenia (Cochrane Schizophrenia Group's Study‐Based Register of Trials) (16 julio 2015 y abril 2017). Este registro se compone de búsquedas exhaustivas de ensayos controlados aleatorizados en muchas bases de datos electrónicas, registros de ensayos, actas de congresos y disertaciones. Las referencias de todos los estudios identificados se investigaron en busca de citas de ensayos adicionales.

Criterios de selección

Se incluyeron los informes identificados mediante la búsqueda si eran de ensayos controlados que incluían personas con DT inducida por antipsicóticos y enfermedades mentales crónicas, que habían sido asignadas al azar a un agente colinérgico o a un placebo o a ninguna intervención. Dos autores de la revisión evaluaron de forma independiente la calidad metodológica de los estudios incluidos.

Obtención y análisis de los datos

Dos revisores extrajeron los datos y, cuando fue posible, calcularon los cocientes de riesgos (CR) o las diferencias de medias (DM), con intervalos de confianza (IC) del 95%. Los datos se analizaron sobre la base de intención de tratar (intention‐to‐treat), con la suposición de que las personas que abandonaron prematuramente no presentaron mejoría. Se evaluó el riesgo de sesgo y se creó una tabla "Resumen de los hallazgos" con la metodología GRADE.

Resultados principales

Se incluyeron 14 estudios que investigaron el uso de fármacos colinérgicos en comparación con placebo publicados entre 1976 y 2014. Todos los estudios incluyeron un número pequeño de participantes (cinco a 60 personas). Tres estudios que investigaron los nuevos fármacos colinérgicos del Alzheimer para el tratamiento de la DT son nuevos en esta actualización. En general, el riesgo de sesgo en los estudios incluidos fue incierto, debido principalmente a informes deficientes; no se describió el ocultamiento de la asignación, la generación de la secuencia no fue explícita, los estudios no estaban claramente cegados, no se sabe si los datos son incompletos y, a menudo, los datos se informaron de manera deficiente o selectiva.

No se conoce con certeza el efecto de los fármacos colinérgicos nuevos o antiguos sobre ninguna mejoría clínicamente importante de los síntomas de DT en comparación con el placebo; la calidad de la evidencia fue muy baja (RR 0,89; IC del 95%: 0,65 a 1,23; 27 personas, cuatro ECA). Ocho ensayos encontraron que los fármacos colinérgicos pueden hacer poca o ninguna diferencia en el deterioro de los síntomas de DT (evidencia de baja calidad, RR 1,11; IC del 95%: 0,55 a 2,24; 147 personas). De nuevo, debido a la evidencia de muy baja calidad, no se conocen con certeza los efectos sobre el estado mental (RR 0,50; IC del 95%: 0,10 a 2,61; 77 personas, 5 ECA), los eventos adversos (RR 0,56; IC del 95%: 0,15 a 2,14; 106 personas, 4 ECA) y el abandono temprano del estudio (RR 1,09; IC del 95%: 0,56 a 2,10; 288 personas, 12 ECA). Ningún estudio informó sobre la confianza social, la inclusión social, las redes sociales o la calidad de vida personalizada.

Conclusiones de los autores

La DT sigue siendo un grave problema de salud pública. Los efectos clínicos tanto de los fármacos colinérgicos más antiguos como de los nuevos agentes colinérgicos, ahora utilizados para tratar la enfermedad de Alzheimer, no están claros, ya que muy pocos estudios demasiado pequeños dejan muchas preguntas sin respuesta. Los fármacos colinérgicos deben seguir siendo de interés para los investigadores y actualmente tienen poco lugar en el trabajo clínico de rutinario. Sin embargo, con el advenimiento de nuevos agentes colinérgicos que ahora se utilizan para tratar la enfermedad de Alzheimer, existe margen para ensayos más informativos. Si estos nuevos agentes colinérgicos deben investigarse para tratar a las personas con DT, sus efectos deben demostrarse en ensayos aleatorizados grandes, bien diseñados, realizados e informados.

PICO

Population
Intervention
Comparison
Outcome

El uso y la enseñanza del modelo PICO están muy extendidos en el ámbito de la atención sanitaria basada en la evidencia para formular preguntas y estrategias de búsqueda y para caracterizar estudios o metanálisis clínicos. PICO son las siglas en inglés de cuatro posibles componentes de una pregunta de investigación: paciente, población o problema; intervención; comparación; desenlace (outcome).

Para saber más sobre el uso del modelo PICO, puede consultar el Manual Cochrane.

Resumen en términos sencillos

Medicamentos colinérgicos para la discinesia tardía inducida por neurolépticos

Pregunta de la revisión

Los fármacos colinérgicos son útiles para tratar el desagradable efecto secundario ‐disquinesia tardía‐ en personas con esquizofrenia u otros problemas de salud mental similares que están tomando antipsicóticos.

Antecedentes

Los pacientes con esquizofrenia a menudo oyen voces y ven cosas (alucinaciones) y tienen creencias extrañas (delirios). Estos síntomas generalmente se tratan con medicamentos antipsicóticos. Sin embargo, estos medicamentos pueden tener efectos secundarios debilitantes. La discinesia tardía es un movimiento involuntario que causa que la cara, la boca, la lengua y la mandíbula presenten convulsiones, espasmos y muecas. Es causada por el uso a largo plazo o las dosis altas de los fármacos antipsicóticos, es difícil de tratar y puede ser incurable. Se ha propuesto que la discinesia tardía podría deberse a una deficiencia colinérgica. Los fármacos colinérgicos más antiguos, como el deanol, la lecitina y el meclofenoxato, se han utilizado para tratar la discinesia tardía. Se han desarrollado nuevos fármacos colinérgicos, como el donepezilo, la galantamina y la rivastigmina, para el tratamiento de la enfermedad de Alzheimer y también pueden ser prometedores en el tratamiento de la discinesia tardía.

Características del estudio.

Se hicieron búsquedas en el registro de ensayos del Grupo Cochrane de Esquizofrenia (Cochrane Schizophrenia Group) en julio de 2015 y abril de 2017. La revisión incluye 14 estudios que investigan el uso de fármacos colinérgicos en comparación con placebo. Todos los estudios asignaron al azar a un pequeño número de participantes (cinco a 60 personas) con esquizofrenia u otras enfermedades mentales crónicas que también habían desarrollado discinesia tardía inducida por antipsicóticos.

Resultados clave

Se encontró que los efectos de los fármacos colinérgicos más antiguos y más nuevos no están claros, ya que hay muy pocos estudios disponibles y demasiado pequeños que no aportan grandes pruebas y dejan muchas preguntas sin respuesta.

Calidad de la evidencia

La evidencia disponible es débil, limitada y a pequeña escala. No es posible recomendar estos fármacos como tratamiento para la discinesia tardía en base a nuestros hallazgos. Para investigar totalmente si el uso de fármacos colinérgicos tiene algún efecto positivo para las personas con discinesia tardía, tendrían que realizarse estudios bien diseñados, más amplios y a más largo plazo, en particular sobre los nuevos fármacos colinérgicos que se utilizan actualmente para tratar la enfermedad de Alzheimer.

Ben Gray, Investigador Superior Externo, McPin Foundation:

http://mcpin.org/

Authors' conclusions

Implications for practice

1. For the person with tardive dyskinesia (TD)

Given the absence of evidence for efficacy, and given that cholinergic medication is associated with adverse effects, it would be understandable if a person with TD would rather avoid these additional treatments. Should the TD be serious and warrant intervention, the person with the movement disorder or their family may wish to ask for use of one of the more modern cholinergics within a well‐designed, conducted and reported randomised trial.

2. For clinicians

The available data on the efficacy of cholinergic medications for the treatment of antipsychotic‐induced TD are insufficient to recommend their use. Adverse effects associated with some of these compounds are burdensome. Nevertheless, there is a real place for clinician‐driven research, with prescription within the context of a randomised trial and routine data collection on outcomes of relevance to people with TD and their clinicians.

3. For policy makers and funders of studies

This is one of the reviews in the series of Cochrane reviews on treatments for antipsychotic‐induced TD (see Table 1). No evidence is convincing that addition or withdrawal of another drug helps with the symptoms of TD. There are, however, many unanswered questions in this area. This unattractive adverse effect is caused, to a greater or lesser extent, by antipsychotic drugs. Clinicians and researchers should feel responsible enough to continue to try to help it. Those compiling guidance could encourage supportive activity and more research into this neglected area.

Implications for research

1. General

If cholinergic agents are to be investigated, their effects should be demonstrated in well‐designed, conducted and reported randomised controlled clinical trials (Moher 2001).

2. Specific
2.1 Reviews

The excluded studies do contain studies which may be of value in other related reviews (Table 2).

Open in table viewer
Table 2. Excluded studies which are relevant to other reviews

Excluded study

Comparison

Treatment category

Relevant review

#1

#2

Nasrallah 1986

Alpha‐methyl‐p‐tyrosine (AMPT)

versus choline chloride

Amino acid

Organic salt

versus hydroxytryptophan

Amino acid (serotonin precursor)

versus valproic acid

Mood stabilisers

versus L‐DOPA

Amino acid

Lieberman 1988

Benztropine versus bromocriptine

Anticholinergic

Dopamine agonist

Bromocriptine versus haloperidol

Dopamine agonist

Antipsychotic

Nasrallah 1986

Choline chloride

versus L‐DOPA

Organic salt

Amino acid

versus hydroxytryptophan

versus valproic

Anticonvulsant

Jus 1978

Deanol

versus lithium carbonate

Antidepressant

Organic salt

versus placebo

Placebo

Chien 1978

versus sodium valproate

Anticonvulsant

versus oxpertine

Antipsychotic

Nasrallah 1986

Hydroxytryptophan versus L‐DOPA

Amino acid

Amino acid

L‐DOPA versus valproic acid

Anticonvulsant

Jus 1978

Lithium carbonate versus placebo

Mood stabiliser

Placebo

Chien 1978

Oxypertine versus sodium valproate

Antipsychotic

Anticonvulsant

2.2 Trials

From the theoretical background, there is some evidence to suggest that cholinergics may benefit people with antipsychotic‐induced TD. It is possible that cholinergic agents have a small‐to‐moderate effect (positive or negative) that has not, as yet, been detected. The results of this review do not support further investigation of the effects of lecithin and deanol. However, there is a need for well‐designed, conducted and reported randomised trials to evaluate the effects of modern cholinergic agents for the treatment of antipsychotic‐induced TD. Such studies are of importance to people with the problem (see Figure 1) and have long been ignored.

2.2.1 Trial design

a. Use of cross‐over design

Trialists find it difficult to identify people with both TD and schizophrenia to participate in trials. Randomised cross‐over design is used in the hope of improving the power of the study to find outcomes of interest. This design initially asks participants to be randomised to one of the experimental interventions, and then, at a pre‐specified time, to be crossed over to the treatment that they did not at first receive. Conditions with a more stable time course than TD are better suited for cross‐over studies (Fleiss 1984). Further difficulties are the carry‐over effect. Unless cross‐over studies include a mid‐study washout period (where the person is free of treatment before starting the next arm of the study), any effect of cholinergic drugs may continue into the second half placebo arm of the trial – the 'carry‐over effect'. Also, carry‐over may involve the re‐growth or retreat of neuroreceptors. This slow re‐balancing, if started, could continue long after all traces of intervention drugs are gone, so the physiological half life of the experimental treatment may not be the only variable to consider when thinking though the issues of carry‐over. TD is also an unstable condition and people with TD may not remain compliant with medication. All these factors make the arguments for not using cross‐over methodology strong, despite the initial attraction (Armitage 1991; Fleiss 1984; Pocock 1983).

b. Sample size calculation

All studies included in this review had a small sample size (five to 60). However, the results suggest that a larger sample size should be used to provide more precise estimates of effect.

c. Outcomes

Many of the outcomes initially desired when this review was started have not been investigated. In addition to efficacy, it is important to assess the influence on mental state and adverse effects. Finally, a service user consultation informed the addition of outcomes of special importance to patients. We have reconsidered all these outcomes in case they were too ambitious and tried to tailor them to a real‐world pragmatic trial design (see Table 3).

Open in table viewer
Table 3. Suggestions for design of future study

Methods

Allocation: randomised, with sequence generation and concealment of allocation clearly described.
Blindness: double, tested.
Duration: 12 months beyond end of intervention at least.
Raters: independent.

Participants

People with antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia.*
Age: any.
Sex: both.
History: any.
N = 300.**

Interventions

Specific cholinergic drug (N = 150) versus placebo (N = 150)

Outcomes

Tardive dyskinesia: any clinically important improvement in TD, any improvement, deterioration.***
Adverse effects: no clinically significant extrapyramidal adverse effects ‐ any time period***, use of any antiparkinsonism drugs, other important adverse events.
Leaving the study early.
Service outcomes: admitted, number of admissions, length of hospitalisation, contacts with psychiatric services.
Compliance with drugs.
Economic evaluations: cost‐effectiveness, cost‐benefit.
General state: relapse, frequency and intensity of minor and major exacerbations.
Social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life: binary measure
Distress among relatives: binary measure.
Burden on family: binary measure.

Notes

* This could be diagnosed by clinical decision. If funds were permitting all participants could be screened using operational criteria, otherwise a random sample should suffice.

** Size of study with sufficient power to highlight about a 10% difference between groups for primary outcome.
*** Primary outcome. The same applies to the measure of primary outcome as for diagnosis. Not everyone may need to have operational criteria applied if clinical impression is proved to be accurate.

Summary of findings

Open in table viewer
Summary of findings for the main comparison. CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO for antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia

CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO for antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia

Patient or population: people with various psychiatric disorders (mainly schizophrenia) and antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia
Settings: mostly inpatients in Australia, Canada, Germany, Japan, Switzerland and the USA.
Intervention: CHOLINERGIC DRUGS (deanol, donepezil, galantamine, meclofenoxate, lecithin) versus PLACEBO

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of Participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

PLACEBO

CHOLINERGIC DRUGS

Tardive dyskinesia: No clinically important improvement

follow‐up: 2 to 12 weeks

1000 per 1000

890 per 1000
(650 to 1000)

RR 0.89
(0.65 to 1.23)

27
(4 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (deanol, donepezil, lecithin) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Tardive dyskinesia: Deterioration

follow‐up: 9 days to 12 weeks

116 per 1000

129 per 1000
(64 to 260)

RR 1.11
(0.55 to 2.24)

147
(8 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
low1,3

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (deanol, donepezil, lecithin, meclofenoxate) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Mental state: Deterioration

follow‐up: 11 days to 12 weeks

56 per 1000

28 per 1000
(6 to 145)

RR 0.50
(0.10 to 2.61)

77
(5 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (deanol, lecithin) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Adverse effects: Any adverse events

follow‐up: 9 days to 8 weeks

98 per 1000

55 per 1000
(15 to 210)

RR 0.56
(0.15 to 2.14)

106
(4 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (donepezil, lecithin, meclofenoxate) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Acceptability of treatment: Leaving the study early

follow‐up: 9 days to 12 weeks

90 per 1000

98 per 1000
(50 to 188)

RR 1.09
(0.56 to 2.10)

288
(12 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,3,4

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (deanol, donepezil, galantamine, meclofenoxate, lecithin) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life ‐ not reported

None of the included studies reported on this outcome.

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1 Downgraded one step for risk of bias: for many studies it was unclear whether randomisation procedure and allocation concealment were carried out adequately.
2 Downgraded two steps for imprecision: very few events and participants, wide CIs that include both no effect and appreciable benefit for the intervention.
3 Downgraded one step for imprecision: wide CIs that include appreciable benefit for both the intervention and the control group, as well as no effect.
4 Downgraded one step for indirectness: leaving the study early is not a direct measure of acceptability of the intervention.

Background

Description of the condition

Since the 1950s antipsychotic (neuroleptic) medication has been used extensively to treat people with chronic mental illnesses such as schizophrenia. These drugs can effectively control symptoms such as abnormal perceptions (hallucinations), disordered thinking and fixed false beliefs (delusions). In addition, maintenance therapy with antipsychotics is associated with a reduced risk of relapse (Schooler 1993). However, antipsychotic medication has also been associated with a wide range of adverse effects, including movement disorders. The appearance of these disorders can be extremely disfiguring, compounds stigma, and is associated with poor compliance to antipsychotic treatment (Barnes 1993; Tarsy 2011).

Tardive dyskinesia (TD) is one such movement disorder and is characterised by abnormal, repetitive and involuntary movements. The clinical features include: tongue protrusion, side‐to‐side or rotatory movement of the jaw, lip smacking, puckering and pursing, and rapid eye blinking (Casey 1999). In some people rapid movements of the arms, legs, and trunk may also occur. TD is a chronic condition of insidious onset, the severity of which fluctuates spontaneously (APA 1992). Although the most frequent cause of TD is the use of antipsychotic medication, it is striking that dose reduction can lead to a temporary exacerbation in symptoms. Conversely, increasing the dose is often associated with a temporary remission (Cavallaro 1993; Smith 1980). Studies on the natural history of TD have reported widely variable remission rates (1% to 62%) depending on the patient's age, psychiatric diagnosis, course of the psychiatric disorder, and duration of therapy (Bergen 1989; Fernandez 2001; Glazer 1990).

The exact mechanisms of the pathophysiology of TD are unknown. Antipsychotic drugs block certain chemical receptor sites in the brain ‐ one of these is specific for dopamine (Casey 1995). One hypothesis explaining the cause of antipsychotic‐induced TD is that long‐term blockade of the dopamine receptors may lead to an imbalance between the activity of dopamine cells and others that employ choline (Alphs 1983; Casey 1995). This theory implies that there is an over activity of dopaminergic transmission in relation to cholinergic transmission in the striatum. Another hypothesis suggests that the chronic use of antipsychotics may also cause an abnormal production of highly active atoms and chemical groups (cytotoxic free radicals), which may damage specific cells in the brain. This, in turn, could contribute to the pathophysiology of TD (Andreassen 2000; Andreassen 2001; Cadet 1989). The latter theory is supported by the persistent nature of the syndrome, once established.

TD occurs in more than 20% of people that use first‐generation antipsychotic drugs continually for longer than three months (Kane 1982; Glazer 2000; Tarsy 2011). Every year 4% to 5% of adults and 25% to 30% of elderly persons who continually use these drugs begin to show signs of TD (APA 1992; Correll 2004). Advancing age is a risk factor for both TD's prevalence and severity, with those who are < 60 years of age being three times more likely to spontaneously remit (Jeste 2000; Smith 1980).

When the second‐generation antipsychotic drugs were introduced in the 1990’s many hoped that they would not cause TD (Miller 2007; Rosenheck 2007). Although the risk of developing TD with second‐generation antipsychotic drugs does seem to be reduced, TD risks have not been eliminated (Miller 2007; Tarsy 2011). There is even some evidence to indicate that rates of TD do not differ at all between first‐ and second‐generation antipsychotic drugs (Leucht 2009; Rosenheck 2007; Woods 2010). The large, definitive, US randomised trial of antipsychotic treatments for schizophrenia (CATIE), with a four‐year period of follow‐up, obtained an incidence rate of TD of around 17% (Miller 2008). Due to widespread use of second‐generation antipsychotic drugs, increased off‐label use, and an ageing population, the frequency of TD is likely to be higher than thought (Cloud 2014; Maher 2012), and increasing. The problem will be considerably greater for people in countries where use of newer drugs is less prevalent (Ballesteros 2000; Martins 2011).

Description of the intervention

Medications affecting the cholinergic pathway have been tested for the treatment of TD due to the hypothesis that TD is correlated with damage in cholinergic cells in striatal subregions. This damage might be caused by changes in dopaminergic transmission, as a result of prolonged antipsychotic treatment (Grimm 2001). A variety of cholinergic medications have been used in TD, starting from the 1970's. The cholinergic compounds included in this review have various functions in relation to acetylcholine, the main neurotransmitter and neuromodulator in the cholinergic pathway.

How the intervention might work

This review looked at compounds such as physostigmine, choline, and lecithin (containing phosphatidyl choline), which were some of the early cholinergic medications used for TD, as they were thought to be of potential help in treating TD by enhancing acetylcholine synthesis (Wurtman 1978). It was thought that high levels of choline in the blood (which, for instance, phosphatidyl choline converts into after digestion) would enhance acetylcholine synthesis in neurons by making the precursor more available. Other treatments included, such as deanol and meclofenoxate, have been tested due to their cholinomimetic actions (Casey 1975; Izumi 1986). Deanol, a synthetic substance, is also thought to be a precursor of acetylcholine, but this is unproven, and deanol might be an acetylcholine agonist or even a suppressor of acetylcholine synthesis. During the last 20 years many new central nervous system (CNS)‐active cholinergic compounds, predominantly acetylcholinesterase‐inhibitors such as galantamine (Loy 2006), rivastigmine (Birks 2015) and donepezil (Birks 2006b), have emerged for the treatment of Alzheimer's disease and dementia. These compounds were also included in this review, due to their potential for treating TD symptoms (Caroff 2001).

Why it is important to do this review

Several atypical antipsychotic drugs have been produced in the last decades that claim to cause less or no TD (Lieberman 1996). These claims may or may not be true, and certainly evidence does point to the fact that thoughtful use of older generation drugs is not associated with any more problems of TD than with newer treatments (Chouinard 2008). However, in a global context, it is likely that the less expensive and more familiar drugs ‐ such as chlorpromazine or haloperidol ‐ will continue to be the mainstay of treatment of people with schizophrenia (WHO Essential List 2010). Use of drugs such as these is associated with the emergence of TD and, therefore, this condition will remain a problem for years to come.

TD can result in considerable social and physical disability (Barnes 1993) and symptoms are often irreversible (Bergen 1989; Fernandez 2001; Gerlach 1988; Glazer 1990). Additionally, TD is frequently associated with lower quality of life (Ascher‐Svanum 2008) and a greater mortality rate (Chong 2009). Given the high incidence and prevalence of TD among people taking antipsychotic medication, the need for prevention or treatment is clear. Unfortunately, there has been sparse evidence to guide clinicians (NICE 2014; Taylor 2009). Although many treatments have been tested, no one intervention has been shown clearly to be effective. Cessation or reduction of the dose of antipsychotic medication would be the ideal management for TD. In clinical practice this is not always possible, not least because in many individuals such a reduction would lead to relapse. This review focuses on whether the addition of different types of cholinergic medications to those already receiving antipsychotic medication is likely to help TD.

This review is one in a series of Cochrane reviews (see Table 1) evaluating treatments for antipsychotic‐induced TD, and is an update of a Cochrane review first published in 1997 (McGrath 1997), and previously updated in 2002 (Tammenmaa 2002).

Objectives

The primary objective of this review is to determine the effects of cholinergic drugs (arecoline, choline, deanol, lecithin, meclofenoxate, physostigmine, RS 86, tacrine, metoxytacrine, galantamine, ipidacrine, donepezil, rivastigmine, eptastigmine, metrifonate, xanomeline, cevimeline) for treating antipsychotic‐induced TD in people with schizophrenia or other chronic mental illness.

The secondary objectives are:
1. to examine whether duration of treatment has an effect on treatment response;
2. to examine whether there is difference in treatment effect for the various cholinergic compounds;
3. to examine whether treatment response differs in people with schizophrenia who are older (above 65 years) and for whom the prevalence of spontaneous dyskinesias is estimated to be higher. (This secondary objective was added in response to a comment from peer review in the first substantial up‐date of the review).

Methods

Criteria for considering studies for this review

Types of studies

All relevant randomised controlled trials. Where a trial was described as 'double‐blind' but it implied that the study was randomised and the demographic details of each group were similar, it was included and we conducted a sensitivity analysis to evaluate the effect of the presence or absence of these data. We excluded quasi‐randomised studies, such as those allocated by using alternate days of the week.

Types of participants

People with schizophrenia or any other serious mental illness, diagnosed by any criteria, irrespective of gender, age or nationality who:
1. required the use of antipsychotics for more than three months;
2. developed TD during antipsychotic treatment (diagnosed by any criteria at baseline of the trial and at least one other occasion);
3. for whom the dose of antipsychotic medication had been stable for one month or more before the trial and during the trial (the same applies for those free of antipsychotics).

Types of interventions

1. The cholinergic drugs

Arecoline, choline, deanol, lecithin, meclofenoxate, physostigmine, RS 86.

In the first substantial update of the review the following cholinergic compounds were assessed to be relevant and added to the scope of the review: 7‐methoxytacrine, cevimeline, donepezil, eptastigmine, galantamine, ipidacrine, metrifonate, rivastigmine, tacrine, xanomeline, .

2. Control condition

Placebo or no intervention.

For the 2017 update a post hoc decision was made to also include studies evaluating the above mentioned cholinergic drugs compared to any other intervention for the treatment of TD.

Types of outcome measures

When appropriate, the we grouped outcomes into time periods ‐ short term (less than six weeks), medium term (between six weeks and six months) and long term (over six months).

Primary outcomes
1. Tardive dyskinesia

No clinically important improvement in the symptoms of individuals, defined as more than 50% improvement on any TD scale ‐ any time period.

2. Adverse effects

No clinically significant extrapyramidal adverse effects ‐ any time period.

Secondary outcomes
1. Tardive dyskinesia

1.1 Any improvement in the symptoms of individuals on any TD scale, as opposed to no improvement.
1.2 Deterioration in the symptoms of individuals, defined as any deleterious change on any TD scale.
1.3 Average change in severity of TD during the trial period.
1.4 Average difference in severity of TD at the end of the trial.

2. Global outcome measures (this category of outcome measures was added in the first substantial update of the review).

2.1 The number of people per treatment group who died for any reason.
2.2 Treatment group mean and standard deviation of endpoint score on any scale of quality of life.
2.3 Treatment group mean and standard deviation of endpoint score on any scale of level of functioning.

3. General mental state changes

3.1 Deterioration in general psychiatric symptoms (such as delusions and hallucinations) defined as any deleterious change on any scale.
3.2 Average difference in severity of psychiatric symptoms at the end of the trial.

4. Acceptability of the treatment

4.1 Acceptability of the intervention to the participant group as measured by numbers of people dropping out during the trial.

5. Adverse effects

5.1 Use of any anti‐parkinsonism drugs.
5.2 Average score/change in extrapyramidal adverse effects.
5.3 Acute dystonia.

6. Other adverse effects, general and specific
7. Hospital and service utilisation outcomes

7.1 Hospital admission.
7.2 Average change in days in hospital.
7.3 Improvement in hospital status (for example: change from formal to informal admission status, use of seclusion, level of observation).

8. Economic outcomes

8.1 Average change in total cost of medical and mental health care.
8.2 Total indirect and direct costs.

9. Social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life measures

9.1. No significant change in social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life measures.
9.2 Average score/change in social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life measures.

10. Behaviour

10.1 Clinically significant agitation.
10.2 Use of adjunctive medication for sedation.
10.3 Aggression to self or others.

11. Cognitive state

11.1 No clinically important change.
11.2 No change, general and specific.

'Summary of findings' table

We used the GRADE approach to interpret findings (Schünemann 2008) and used GRADEpro to export data from this review to create a 'Summary of findings' table. This table provides outcome‐specific information concerning the overall quality of evidence from each included study in the comparison, the magnitude of effect of the interventions examined, and the sum of the available data on all outcomes we rated as important to patient care and decision making. We selected the following main outcomes for inclusion in the 'Summary of findings' table.

1. Tardive dyskinesia
1.1 Improved to a clinically important extent
1.2 Deteriorated

2. Mental state
2.1 Deteriorated

3. Adverse effect
3.1 Any adverse event
3.2 Adverse effects: no clinically significant extrapyramidal adverse effects

4. Acceptability of treatment
4.1 Leaving the study early

5. Social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life measures*
5.1 No significant change in social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life measures for either recipients of care or caregivers.

* Outcome designated important to patients. We wished to add perspectives from people’s personal experience with TD to the research agenda. A consultation with service users was planned where a previously published version of a review in the Cochrane TD series (Soares‐Weiser 2011; see Table 1) and a lay overview of that review gave the foundation for the discussions. The session was planned to provide time to reflect on current research on TD and consider gaps in knowledge. The report is published in the Health Technology Assessment (HTA) report for the UK National Institute of Health Research (Bergman 2017). We have added one figure showing one service user's expression of frustration concerning this neglected area of research (Figure 1). Informed by the results of the consultation, for this review, we updated outcomes for the 'Summary of findings' table.


Message from one of the participants of the Public and patient involvement consultation of service user perspectives on tardive dyskinesia research.

Message from one of the participants of the Public and patient involvement consultation of service user perspectives on tardive dyskinesia research.

Search methods for identification of studies

Electronic searches

The 2017 review update was carried out in parallel with updating eight other TD reviews, see Table 1 for details. The search for trials covered all nine TD reviews.

1. Cochrane Schizophrenia Group’s Study‐Based Register of Trials

We searched the register on 16 July, 2015 and 26 April 2017 using the following string:

*Tardive Dyskinesia* in Healthcare Condition Field of Study

In such a study‐based register, searching the major concept retrieves all the synonym keywords and relevant studies because all the studies have already been organised based on their interventions and linked to the relevant topics.

The Cochrane Schizophrenia Group’s Register of Trials is compiled by systematic searches of major resources (including AMED, BIOSIS, CINAHL, Embase, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, PubMed, and registries of clinical trials) and their monthly updates, handsearches, grey literature, and conference proceedings (see Group’s Module). There is no language, date, document type, or publication status limitations for inclusion of records into the register.

2. Details of previous electronic searches

See Appendix 1.

Searching other resources

1. Reference searching

We inspected references of all included studies for further relevant studies.

2. Personal contact

Where necessary, we contacted the first author of each included study for information regarding unpublished trials. We noted the outcome of this contact in the included studies table.

Data collection and analysis

Selection of studies

For the 2017 update, review author RA and Antonio Grande (AG) (see Acknowledgements) inspected all abstracts of studies identified as above and identified potentially relevant reports. We resolved disagreements by discussion, or where there was still doubt, we acquired the full‐text article for further inspection. We acquired the full‐text articles of all relevant reports/abstracts meeting the initial criteria for reassessment and carefully inspected for a final decision on inclusion (see Criteria for considering studies for this review). RA and AG were not blinded to the names of the authors, institutions or journal of publication. Where difficulties or disputes arose, we asked review author HB for assistance and had it not been possible to decide, or if adequate information was not available to make a decision, we planned to add these studies to those awaiting assessment and contact the authors of the papers for clarification.

Data extraction and management

1. Extraction

For the 2017 update, review authors RA and HB independently extracted data from all included studies. Again, we discussed any disagreement and documented decisions. With remaining problems KSW helped clarify issues and we documented these final decisions. We extracted data presented only in graphs and figures whenever possible, but included only if two review authors independently had the same result. We attempted to contact authors through an open‐ended request in order to obtain missing information or for clarification whenever necessary. If studies were multicentre, where possible, we extracted data relevant to each component centre separately.

2. Management
2.1 Forms

For the 2017 update we extracted data to simple forms. Extracted data are available here with a link to the original source PDF for each item.

2.2 Scale‐derived data

We included continuous data from rating scales only if:

a) the psychometric properties of the measuring instrument have been described in a peer‐reviewed journal (Marshall 2000); and
b) the measuring instrument has not been written or modified by one of the trialists for that particular trial.

Ideally, the measuring instrument should either be i. a self‐report or ii. completed by an independent rater or relative (not the therapist). We realise that this is not often reported clearly; we noted in Description of studies if this was the case or not.

2.3 Endpoint versus change data

There are advantages of both endpoint and change data. Change data can remove a component of between‐person variability from the analysis. On the other hand, calculation of change needs two assessments (baseline and endpoint), which can be difficult in unstable and difficult to measure conditions such as schizophrenia. We decided primarily to use endpoint data, and only use change data if the former were not available. We combined endpoint and change data in the analysis as we preferred to use mean differences (MD) rather than standardised mean differences (SMD) throughout (Higgins 2011).

2.4 Skewed data

Continuous data on clinical and social outcomes are often not normally distributed. To avoid the pitfall of applying parametric tests to non‐parametric data, we applied the following standards to relevant data before inclusion.

We entered data from studies of at least 200 participants in the analysis, because skewed data pose less of a problem in large studies. We also entered all relevant change data as when continuous data are presented on a scale that includes a possibility of negative values (such as change data), it is difficult to tell whether data are skewed or not.

For endpoint data from studies < 200 participants:

(a) when a scale started from the finite number zero, we subtracted the lowest possible value from the mean, and divided this by the standard deviation (SD). If this value was lower than 1, it strongly suggests a skew and we excluded these data. If this ratio was higher than 1 but below 2, there is suggestion of skew. We entered these data and tested whether its inclusion or exclusion changed the results substantially. Finally, if the ratio was larger than 2, we included these data, because skew is less likely (Altman 1996; Higgins 2011).

(b) if a scale starts from a positive value (such as the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), (Kay 1986)), which can have values from 30 to 210), we modified the calculation described above to take the scale starting point into account. In these cases skew is present if 2 SD > (S‐S min), where S is the mean score and 'S min' is the minimum score.

2.5 Common measure

Where relevant, to facilitate comparison between trials, we converted variables that can be reported in different metrics, such as days in hospital (mean days per year, per week or per month) to a common metric (e.g. mean days per month).

2.6 Conversion of continuous to binary

Where possible, we converted continuous outcome measures to dichotomous data. This can be done by identifying cut‐off points on rating scales and dividing participants accordingly into 'clinically improved' or 'not clinically improved'. It is generally assumed that if there is a 50% reduction in a scale‐derived score such as the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS, Overall 1962) or the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS, Kay 1986), this can be considered as a clinically significant response (Leucht 2005; Leucht 2005a). If data based on these thresholds were not available, we used the primary cut‐off presented by the original authors.

2.7 Direction of graphs

Where possible, we entered data in such a way that the area to the left of the line of no effect indicated a favourable outcome for cholinergic medication. Where keeping to this made it impossible to avoid outcome titles with clumsy double‐negatives (e.g. 'Not un‐improved'), we presented data where the left of the line indicates an unfavourable outcome and noted this in the relevant graphs.

Assessment of risk of bias in included studies

Review authors RA and HB independently assessed risk of bias within the included studies by using criteria described in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions to assess trial quality (Higgins 2011). This set of criteria is based on evidence of associations between overestimate of effect and high risk of bias of the article such as sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding, incomplete outcome data and selective reporting.

If the raters disagreed, we made the final rating by consensus, with the involvement of another member of the review group. Where inadequate details of randomisation and other characteristics of trials were provided, we contacted authors of the studies in order to obtain further information. If non‐concurrence occurred, we reported this.

Measures of treatment effect

1. Binary data

For binary outcomes we calculated a standard estimation of the risk ratio (RR) and its 95% confidence interval (CI). It has been shown that RR is more intuitive (Boissel 1999) than odds ratios as odds ratios tend to be interpreted as RR by clinicians (Deeks 2000).

2. Continuous data

For continuous outcomes we estimated mean difference (MD) between groups. We preferred not to calculate effect size measures (standardised mean difference SMD). However, if scales of very considerable similarity were used, we presumed there was a small difference in measurement, and calculated effect size and transformed the effect back to the units of one or more of the specific instruments.

Unit of analysis issues

1. Cluster trials

Studies increasingly employ 'cluster randomisation' (such as randomisation by clinician or practice), but analysis and pooling of clustered data poses problems. Firstly, authors often fail to account for intra‐class correlation in clustered studies, leading to a 'unit of analysis' error (Divine 1992) whereby P values are spuriously low, confidence intervals unduly narrow and statistical significance overestimated. This causes type I errors (Bland 1997; Gulliford 1999).

If any of the included trials had randomised participants by clusters, and where clustering had not been accounted for in primary studies, we would have presented such data in a table, with a (*) symbol to indicate the presence of a probable unit of analysis error. In subsequent versions of this review, we will seek to contact first authors of studies to obtain intra‐class correlation coefficients (ICCs) for their clustered data and adjust for this by using accepted methods (Gulliford 1999). Where clustering has been incorporated into the analysis of primary studies, we will present these data as if from a non‐cluster randomised study, but adjust for the clustering effect.

We have sought statistical advice and have been advised that the binary data as presented in a report should be divided by a 'design effect'. This is calculated using the mean number of participants per cluster (m) and the ICC [Design effect = 1+(m‐1)*ICC] (Donner 2002). If the ICC is not reported it will be assumed to be 0.1 (Ukoumunne 1999).

If cluster studies have been appropriately analysed taking into account ICCs and relevant data documented in the report, synthesis with other studies would be possible using the generic inverse variance technique.

2. Cross‐over trials

A major concern of cross‐over trials is the carry‐over effect. It occurs if an effect (e.g. pharmacological, physiological or psychological) of the treatment in the first phase is carried over to the second phase. As a consequence, on entry to the second phase the participants can differ systematically from their initial state despite a wash‐out phase. For the same reason cross‐over trials are not appropriate if the condition of interest is unstable (Elbourne 2002). As both effects are very likely in severe mental illness, we only used data of the first phase of cross‐over studies.

3. Studies with multiple treatment groups

Where a study involved more than two treatment arms, if relevant, we presented the additional treatment arms in comparisons. If data were binary we simply added and combined within the two‐by‐two table. If data were continuous, we combined data following the formula in section 7.7.3.8  (Combining groups) of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011). We did not use data where the additional treatment arms were not relevant.

Dealing with missing data

1. Overall loss of credibility

At some degree of loss of follow‐up, data must lose credibility (Xia 2009). We chose that, for any particular outcome, should more than 50% of data be unaccounted for, we would not reproduce these data or use them within analyses. If, however, more than 50% of those in one arm of a study were lost, but the total loss was less than 50%, we addressed this within the 'Summary of findings' table by down‐rating quality. We also planned to downgrade quality within the 'Summary of findings' table should loss be 25% to 50% in total.

2. Binary

In the case where attrition for a binary outcome was between 0% and 50% and where these data were not clearly described, we presented data on a 'once‐randomised‐always‐analyse' basis (an intention‐to‐treat (ITT) analysis). We assumed all those leaving the study early had no improvement. We undertook a sensitivity analysis to test how prone the primary outcomes were to change by comparing data only from people who completed the study to that point to the ITT analysis using the above assumptions.

3. Continuous
3.1 Attrition

We reported and used data where attrition for a continuous outcome was between 0% and 50%, and data only from people who completed the study to that point were reported.

3.2 Standard deviations

If standard deviations (SDs) were not reported, we first tried to obtain the missing values from the authors. If not available, where there were missing measures of variance for continuous data, but an exact standard error (SE) and confidence intervals available for group means, and either 'P' value or 't' value available for differences in mean, we calculated them according to the rules described in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011): When only the SE is reported, SDs are calculated by the formula SD = SE * square root (n). Chapters 7.7.3 and 16.1.3 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011) present detailed formulae for estimating SDs from P values, t or F values, confidence intervals, ranges or other statistics. If these formulae did not apply, we calculated the SDs according to a validated imputation method which is based on the SDs of the other included studies (Furukawa 2006). Although some of these imputation strategies can introduce error, the alternative would be to exclude a given study’s outcome and thus to lose information. We nevertheless examined the validity of the imputations in a sensitivity analysis excluding imputed values.

3.3 Assumptions about participants who left the trials early or were lost to follow‐up

Various methods are available to account for participants who left the trials early or were lost to follow‐up. Some trials just present the results of study completers, others use the method of last observation carried forward (LOCF), while more recently methods such as multiple imputation or mixed‐effects models for repeated measurements (MMRM) have become more of a standard. While the latter methods seem to be somewhat better than LOCF (Leon 2006), we feel that the high percentage of participants leaving the studies early and differences in the reasons for leaving the studies early between groups is often the core problem in randomised schizophrenia trials. We therefore did not exclude studies based on the statistical approach used. However, we preferred to use the more sophisticated approaches. (e.g. MMRM or multiple‐imputation) and only presented completer analyses if some kind of ITT data were not available at all. Moreover, we addressed this issue in the item "incomplete outcome data" of the 'Risk of bias' tool.

Assessment of heterogeneity

1. Clinical heterogeneity

We considered all included studies initially, without seeing comparison data, to judge clinical heterogeneity. We simply inspected all studies for clearly outlying people or situations which we had not predicted would arise and discussed them in the text if they arose.

2. Methodological heterogeneity

We considered all included studies initially, without seeing comparison data, to judge methodological heterogeneity. We simply inspected all studies for clearly outlying methods which we had not predicted would arise and discussed them in the text if they arose.

3. Statistical heterogeneity
3.1 Visual inspection

We visually inspected graphs to investigate the possibility of statistical heterogeneity.

3.2 Employing the I2 statistic

We investigated heterogeneity between studies by considering the I2 method alongside the Chi2 'P' value. The I2 provides an estimate of the percentage of inconsistency thought to be due to chance (Higgins 2003). The importance of the observed value of I2 depends on i. magnitude and direction of effects and ii. strength of evidence for heterogeneity (e.g. 'P' value from Chi2  test, or a confidence interval for I2). An I2 estimate greater than or equal to around 50% accompanied by a statistically significant Chi2 statistic, can be interpreted as evidence of substantial levels of heterogeneity (Section 9.5.2 Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of InterventionsHiggins 2011). We explored and discussed in the text potential reasons for substantial levels of heterogeneity (Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity).

Assessment of reporting biases

Reporting biases arise when the dissemination of research findings is influenced by the nature and direction of results (Egger 1997). These are described in Section 10 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011). We are aware that funnel plots may be useful in investigating reporting biases but are of limited power to detect small‐study effects. We did not use funnel plots for outcomes where there were 10 or fewer studies, or where all studies were of similar sizes. In future versions of this review, if funnel plots are possible, we will seek statistical advice in their interpretation.

Data synthesis

We understand that there is no closed argument for preference for use of fixed‐effect or random‐effects models. The random‐effects method incorporates an assumption that the different studies are estimating different, yet related, intervention effects. This often seems to be true to us and the random‐effects model takes into account differences between studies even if there is no statistically significant heterogeneity. There is, however, a disadvantage to the random‐effects model. It puts added weight onto small studies, which often are the most biased ones. Depending on the direction of effect, these studies can either inflate or deflate the effect size. We chose the fixed‐effect model for all analyses.

Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity

1. Subgroup analyses
1.1 Duration of treatment

We anticipated a subgroup analysis to examine whether any improvement occurred with short periods of intervention (less than six weeks) and, if this did occur, whether this effect was maintained at longer periods of follow‐up.

1.2 Cholinergic compound

As different cholinergic drug compounds may have differential effects on antipsychotic‐induced TD, we performed a subgroup analysis to compare the effects of different cholinergic drugs. We proposed to undertake comparisons only for primary outcomes to minimise the risk of multiple comparisons.

1.3 Older participants

We also wanted to examine whether the treatment response differs in people with schizophrenia who are older (above 65 years) and for whom the prevalence of spontaneous dyskinesias is estimated to be higher. We had hoped to present data for this subgroup for the primary outcomes.

2. Investigation of heterogeneity

We reported inconsistency if it was high. First, we investigated whether data were entered correctly. Second, if data were correct, we visually inspected the graph and successively removed outlying studies to see if homogeneity was restored. For this review we decided that should this occur with data contributing to the summary finding of no more than around 10% of the total weighting, we would present data. If not, we would not pool such data but discuss relevant issues. We know of no supporting research for this 10% cut‐off but are investigating use of prediction intervals as an alternative to this unsatisfactory state.

When unanticipated clinical or methodological heterogeneity were obvious, we simply discussed these. We did not undertake sensitivity analyses relating to these.

Sensitivity analysis

1. Implication of randomisation

If trials were described in some way as to imply randomisation we undertook a sensitivity analyses for the primary outcomes. We included these studies in the analyses and if there was no substantive difference when the implied randomised studies were added to those with better description of randomisation, then we used relevant data from these studies.

2. Assumptions for lost binary data

Where assumptions had to be made regarding people lost to follow‐up (see Dealing with missing data), we compared the findings of the primary outcomes when we used our assumption compared with completer data only. If there was a substantial difference, we reported and discussed these results, but continued to employ our assumption.

Where assumptions had to be made regarding missing SDs data (see Dealing with missing data), we compared the findings on primary outcomes when we used our assumption compared with completer data only. We undertook a sensitivity analysis to test how prone results were to change when 'completer' data only were compared to the imputed data using the above assumption. If there was a substantial difference, we reported and discussed these results, but continued to employ our assumption.

3. Risk of bias

We analysed the effects of excluding trials that we judged to be at high risk of bias across one or more of the domains of randomisation (implied as randomised with no further details available) allocation concealment, blinding and outcome reporting for the meta‐analysis of the primary outcome. If the exclusion of trials at high risk of bias did not substantially alter the direction of effect or the precision of the effect estimates, we included data from these trials in the analysis

4. Imputed values

Had we included cluster trials, we would have undertaken a sensitivity analysis to assess the effects of including data from trials where we used imputed values for ICC in calculating the design effect.

If we found substantial differences in the direction or precision of effect estimates in any of the sensitivity analyses listed above, we did not pool data from the excluded trials with the other trials contributing to the outcome, but presented them separately.

5. Fixed and random effects

We synthesised data using a fixed‐effect model, however, we also synthesised data for the primary outcome using a random‐effects model to evaluate whether this altered the significance of the results.

Results

Description of studies

Please see Characteristics of included studies and Characteristics of excluded studies.

Results of the search

The 2015 and 2017 update searches were part of an update of nine Cochrane reviews, see Table 1. The 2015 search retrieved 704 references for 344 studies, see Figure 2 for study flow diagram. After having excluded irrelevant references at title and abstract screening, we screened full texts of 72 references (63 studies). Three of these reports were new included studies to this review (Caroff 2007; Jahanian 2014; Ogunmefun 2009), added to the 11 already included studies. One of these studies (Caroff 2007) was an ongoing study in the previously published version of this review. We were able to exclude four studies that were awaiting assessment in the previously published version of this review (Gelenberg 1989; Joe 1985; Marsalek 1994; Perez Cruet 1981), and we excluded one more study for this update (Bartels 1981), added to the 44 already excluded studies from the previously published version of this review. No studies await assessment, and as far as we are aware there are no ongoing studies that would be relevant to this review.


Study flow diagram for 2015 and 2017 searches.

Study flow diagram for 2015 and 2017 searches.

The 2017 search found eight records (five studies). The editorial base of the Cochrane Schizophrenia Group screened these records and no new studies were relevant to this review. They could be relevant to the other reviews in this series of TD reviews (see Table 1), and were put into awaiting assessment of the miscellaneous treatments review (Soares‐Weiser 2003).

Included studies

Overall, the review now includes 14 studies with 364 participants published between 1976 and 2014. Three included studies are new to this update (Caroff 2007; Jahanian 2014; Ogunmefun 2009).

1. Methods

All studies were stated to be randomised and double‐blinded. For further details, please see sections below on Allocation (selection bias) and Blinding (performance bias and detection bias).

2. Design

All included studies presented a parallel longitudinal design. Eight of the 14 studies used a cross‐over design with two periods (Caroff 2007; Gelenberg 1990; Jackson 1978; Jackson 1979; Kocher 1980; Lucius 1976; Ogunmefun 2009; Tarsy 1977). We had considered this as likely when embarking on the review and have used only the data from before the first cross‐over for the reasons outlined above (Unit of analysis issues).

3. Duration

TD is often a chronic condition and symptoms tend to fluctuate and show considerable variability across time. Only four studies, however, all cross‐over trials, had a duration longer than six weeks (Caroff 2007 (12 weeks first treatment phase, 30 weeks total duration); Gelenberg 1990 (eight weeks first treatment phase, 18 to 20 weeks total duration); Jackson 1978 (12 weeks first treatment phase, 32 weeks total duration); Tarsy 1977 (eight weeks first treatment phase, 16 weeks total duration)). All the other included studies were of short duration (nine days to six weeks).

4. Participants

Participants now total 364 people. Both sexes were recruited and the age range was wide, though most people were men in their 50s. Diagnoses included various chronic psychiatric disorders, but mainly schizophrenia. All had antipsychotic‐induced TD diagnosed using Schooler and Kane’s research diagnostic criteria, or by a clinical psychiatrist. The number of participants ranged from five to 60 (median 21).

5. Setting

Most trials were conducted in hospital. Only two studies specifically recruited outpatients (Gelenberg 1990; Tarsy 1977). The studies themselves were from around the world, with eight conducted in the USA (Beckham 1981; Caroff 2007; Gelenberg 1990; Jackson 1978; Jackson 1979; Ogunmefun 2009; Price 1982; Tarsy 1977), and one each in Japan (Yagi 1990), Canada (de Montigny 1979), Australia (George 1981), Iran (Jahanian 2014), Switzerland (Kocher 1980) and Germany (Lucius 1976).

6. Interventions
6.1 Cholinergic drugs

6.1.1 Deanol

Six studies (de Montigny 1979; George 1981; Jackson 1978; Kocher 1980; Lucius 1976; Tarsy 1977) used deanol in doses ranging from 600 mg/day to 2000 mg/day.

6.1.2 Lecithin

Four studies (Beckham 1981; Gelenberg 1990; Jackson 1979; Price 1982) used lecithin in doses ranging from 50 g/day to 60 g/day which contained 20 g/day to 35g/day phosphatidyl choline (a theoretical precursor of acetylcholine).

6.1.3 Meclofenoxate hydrochloride

Yagi 1990 applied meclofenoxate hydrochloride in a dose of 900 mg/day.

6.1.4 Galantamine

Caroff 2007 used galantamine in doses increasing to 12 mg twice per day over 12 weeks.

6.1.5 Rivastigmine

Jahanian 2014 used rivastigmine in a dose of 1.5 mg twice per day.

6.1.6 Donepezil

Ogunmefun 2009 used donepezil in doses ranging from 5 mg to 10 mg per day.

6.2 Comparison group

In all studies placebo was used as a comparison group, with no further details given. In one study a comparison between two doses of deanol was also made (George 1981). None of the included studies compared cholinergic drugs with another active intervention.

Participants remained on schizophrenia treatment antipsychotic medication during the trials.

7. Outcomes
7.1 General

Some outcomes were presented in graphs, with inexact P values of differences, or a statement of significant or non‐significant difference. This made it impossible to acquire raw data for synthesis. Some continuous outcomes could not be extracted due to missing number of participants or missing means, standard deviations, or standard errors. We have shown details of the scales that provided usable data below. We have provided reasons for exclusions of data under 'Outcomes' in the Characteristics of included studies table.

7.2 Scales used to measure TD symptoms

7.2.1 Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS)

The AIMS (Guy 1976a) is a 12‐item scale consisting of a standardised examination followed by questions rating the orofacial, extremity and trunk movements, as well as three global measurements. Each of these 10 items can be scored from zero (none) to four (severe). Two additional items assess the dental status. The AIMS ranges from zero to 40, with higher scores indicating greater severity.

7.2.2 Tardive Dyskinesia Rating Scale (TDRS)

The TDRS (Simpson 1979) is a 34‐item scale consisting of measurement of the movements around the orofacial region, neck, trunk and extremities. Each of these items can be scored from zero (absent) to five (severe). This scale ranges from 10 to 102, with higher scores indicating greater severity.

7.3 Scales used to measure global state

7.3.1 Clinical Global Impression

The CGI is a three‐item scale commonly used in studies on schizophrenia to enable clinicians to quantify severity of illness and overall clinical improvement (Guy 1976b). The items are: severity of illness; global improvement and efficacy index. A seven‐point scoring system is usually employed with low scores indicating decreased severity and/or greater recovery.

7.4 Mental state

Many trials recorded changes in general mental state, and many different ways were employed to rate these changes. No scores, however, were reported in sufficient detail (number of people, mean and SD) to be used in analysis. Only the outcome of 'deterioration of mental state' could be found and used.

7.5 Adverse effects

Possible worsening of acute extrapyramidal symptoms due to cholinergic medication was assessed in some trials, however, trialists did not report scores in detail. All trials assessed general adverse effects, but, again, we often found it impossible to extract useful data (see Characteristics of included studies table); in many cases it was reported that no events occurred. Eight trials assessed specific adverse events, such as gastric adverse events.

Excluded studies

There are 49 excluded studies (53 references). Thirty‐eight of these studies were not randomised and thus excluded. One RCT was excluded because participants were not on a stable dose of antipsychotics before the trial (Simpson 1977). One RCT provided no usable data (Marsalek 1997), and nor did another nine cross‐over RCTs that did not report outcome data for the first phase before crossing over to the next treatment (Chien 1978; Domino 1985; Gelenberg 1989; Joe 1985; Jus 1978; Lieberman 1988; Nasrallah 1986; Penovich 1978; Perez Cruet 1981). We contacted the authors of eight of these studies but received no more details on outcome data. We could not identify up‐to‐date contact details for authors of two of these studies published 35 to 20 years ago (Chien 1978; Marsalek 1997); they were also excluded as we assume it very unlikely to receive data so many years later.

Studies awaiting assessment

There are no studies awaiting assessment.

Ongoing studies

We know of no ongoing studies.

Risk of bias in included studies

Please refer to Figure 3 and Figure 4 for graphical overviews of the risk of bias in the included studies.


'Risk of bias, summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

'Risk of bias, summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.


'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.

Allocation

Ogunmefun 2009 and Tarsy 1977 reported using random number tables, though did not describe allocation concealment. Gelenberg 1990 and Lucius 1976 explicitly described an external allocation procedure. All other studies were not explicit about how the randomisation sequence was generated or how allocation was achieved other than using the word "randomized".

Blinding

Although all studies were conducted on a double‐blind basis, only three (Beckham 1981; Jackson 1979; Price 1982) explicitly described how this was undertaken and no study tested the blindness of raters, clinicians and trial participants. Eight trials (Beckham 1981; George 1981; Jackson 1978; Jackson 1979; Kocher 1980; Lucius 1976; Ogunmefun 2009; Price 1982) adequately described blinding of outcome assessors.

Incomplete outcome data

Two studies (Beckham 1981; Gelenberg 1990) had a drop‐out rate of greater than 30%. These studies were rated as having a high risk of attrition bias. In all cases, however, we tried to ensure that every person randomised was analysed.

Selective reporting

The majority of data in this review originates from published reports. Expected outcomes (impact on TD symptoms) were reported for most of the trials. Only Beckham 1981 and Jahanian 2014 were considered to have a low risk of reporting bias as they reported all outcomes that were stated in their published protocols (Beckham 1981 was a thesis which contained all detailed methods applied). All other studies did not fully report data for the stated outcome measures.

Other potential sources of bias

All studies had small or very small sample sizes (five to 60 participants per study). Eight of the 13 studies used a cross‐over design, and in most studies it was unclear whether other sources of bias may exist.

Effects of interventions

See: Summary of findings for the main comparison CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO for antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia

1. Comparison 1. Cholinergic drugs versus placebo

1.1 TD symptoms

We had chosen 'any improvement in TD symptoms of more than 50% on any TD scale ‐ any time period' as a primary outcome. Although the data we found in trials did not fit this exactly, we feel that the outcome 'not improved to a clinically important extent' fits best with what we had hoped to find.

1.1.1 Not improved to a clinically important extent

There was no significant difference for cholinergic drugs over placebo on clinically important improvement after two to 12 weeks treatment (very low‐quality evidence, 4 trials, 27 people, risk ratio (RR) 0.89, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.65 to 1.23, I2 = 0%, Analysis 1.1).

1.1.2 Not any improvement

For the outcome of 'not any improvement in TD symptoms', we found no difference between cholinergic drugs and placebo after nine days to 12 weeks treatment (low‐quality evidence, 9 trials, 180 people, RR 0.87, 95% CI 0.71 to 1.07, I2=1%, Analysis 1.2). One study also provided self‐reported incidence of not any improvement in TD symptoms and found no difference between cholinergic drugs and placebo after nine to 11 days treatment (1 trial, 30 people, RR 0.92, 95% CI 0.62 to 1.36, Analysis 1.3).

1.1.3 Average endpoint scores

TD symptoms were also measured on the continuous AIMS and TDRS scales (see Description of studies above). No difference was found between cholinergic drugs and placebo on either average AIMS endpoint scores after two to 12 weeks treatment (low‐quality evidence, 7 studies, 171 people, mean difference (MD) ‐0.12, 95% CI ‐0.44 to 0.21, I2 = 44%, Analysis 1.4) or average TDRS endpoint scores after eight weeks treatment (5 participants, 1 study, not estimable, Analysis 1.5).

1.1.4 Deterioration of symptoms

There was also no difference between cholinergic drugs and placebo with regard to deterioration of TD symptoms after nine days to 12 weeks treatment (low‐quality evidence, 8 trials, 147 people, RR 1.11, 95% CI 0.55 to 2.24, I2=0%, Analysis 1.6). One study also provided self‐reported incidence of deterioration in TD symptoms and found no difference between cholinergic drugs and placebo after nine to 11 days treatment (1 trial, 30 people, RR 3.00, 95% CI 0.13 to 68.26, Analysis 1.7).

1.2 Global outcome measures
1.2.1 Death for any reason

No deaths were reported in any of the studies where this information could be extracted (11 trials, 278 people, Analysis 1.8). For cross‐over trials we counted only the first period before the cross‐over. However, in one cross‐over study (Tarsy 1977) one person died suddenly at home due to acute aspiration in the second cross‐over period, his placebo period. By this time he had completed eight weeks of deanol treatment and four weeks of placebo treatment.

1.2.2 Global outcomes

One study assessed global usefulness of meclofenoxate on the categorical Global Usefulness Rating. This study showed no difference between meclofenoxate and placebo after eight weeks treatment (60 people, RR 0.89, 95% CI 0.59 to 1.32, Analysis 1.9).

1.2.3 Global state

Another study assessed global state with the CGI and showed no difference between lecithin and placebo after 11 days treatment (31 people, RR ‐0.43, 95% CI ‐1.36 to 0.50, Analysis 1.10).

1.3 Mental state
1.3.1 Deterioration

There was no significant difference in deterioration of mental state between cholinergic drugs and placebo after 11 days to 12 weeks treatment (very low‐quality evidence, RR 0.50, 95% CI 0.10 to 2.61; 77 participants, 5 studies; I2 = 0%, Analysis 1.11).

1.4 Adverse effects
1.4.1 Any adverse effects

Although four studies (106 participants) reported on any adverse events, only one study reported that any events occurred, 3/31 in the meclofenoxate group compared with 5/29 in the placebo group after eight weeks treatment (very low‐quality evidence, RR 0.56, 95% CI 0.15 to 2.14; 60 participants; Analysis 1.12). The three studies reporting on donepezil or lecithin reported no events.

1.4.2 Various specific adverse effects

Out of eight studies (130 participants) that reported on various specific adverse events, only two studies reported that specific adverse events occurred. For deanol, one study reported that 4/10 participants had gastric adverse events versus 0/10 participants on placebo after three weeks treatment (RR 9.00, 95% CI 0.55 to 147.95; 5 studies, 61 participants; Analysis 1.13), and two studies reported that 8/32 participants had sedation, peripheral cholinergic effects, and undesirable body odour events, versus 0/22 participants on placebo after three to four weeks treatment (RR 6.83, 95% CI 0.99 to 47.25; 6 studies, 94 participants; I2 = 0%; Analysis 1.13). The four other studies reporting on deanol reported that no specific adverse events occurred, and two studies evaluating lecithin reported that no specific adverse events occurred (see Analysis 1.13).

1.5 Leaving the study early

Using cholinergic drugs did not significantly increase the risk of a person leaving the study early after nine days to 12 weeks treatment (very low‐quality evidence, 12 studies, 288 people, RR 1.09, 95% CI 0.56 to 2.10, I2 = 47%, Analysis 1.14).

We did not identify any studies that reported on hospital and service utilisation outcomes, economic outcomes, social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, personalised quality of life, behaviour, or cognitive state.

1.6 Subgroup analysis
1.6.1 Duration of treatment

We stratified analyses by duration. Any effects that cholinergic drugs may have had did not clearly change in relation to duration of treatment.

1.6.2 Cholinergic compound

We stratified analyses by cholinergic compound. Any effects that cholinergic drugs may have did not clearly change in relation to type of compound.

1.6.3 Older participants

It was not possible to evaluate whether participants aged > 65 years responded differently to younger participants, since no trial reported data for different age groups that could be extracted for separate analyses.

1.7 Heterogeneity

Data were mostly homogeneous. We detected statistical heterogeneity as described in Assessment of heterogeneity for two of the outcomes.

  1. TD: average endpoint score on the AIMS (I2 = 44%, P = 0.10): seven studies reported on this outcome, but there is no clear outlier.

    1. Three studies reported an effect estimate favouring cholinergic drug over placebo, but none of the studies reported statistically significant differences between groups (see Analysis 1.4): lecithin versus placebo after two weeks treatment (short term) (MD ‐1.07, 95% CI ‐2.21 to 0.07; 6 participants, 1 RCT), lecithin versus placebo after eight weeks treatment (medium term) (MD ‐0.10, 95% CI ‐1.04 to 0.84; 14 participants, 1 RCT), and meclofenoxate versus placebo after eight weeks treatment (medium term) (MD ‐0.19, 95% CI ‐0.58 to 0.20; 60 participants, 1 RCT).

    2. Four studies reported an effect estimate favouring placebo over cholinergic drug, but again, none of the studies reported statistically significant differences between groups (see Analysis 1.4): rivastigmine versus placebo after eight weeks treatment (short term) (MD 2.20, 95% CI ‐1.16 to 5.56; 40 participants, 1 RCT), deanol versus placebo after 12 weeks treatment (medium term) (MD 1.42, 95% CI ‐0.29 to 3.13; 6 participants, 1 RCT), galantamine versus placebo after 12 weeks treatment (medium term) (MD 1.50, 95% CI ‐0.44 to 3.44; 35 participants, 1 RCT), donepezil versus placebo after four to six weeks treatment (short term) (MD 1.10, 95% CI ‐4.22 to 6.42; 10 participants, 1 RCT).

  2. Acceptability of treatment: leaving the study early (I2 = 47%, P = 0.13): 12 studies reported on this outcome, but only four reported that any events occurred (= that any participants left the study early), and none of these found a statistically significant difference between groups. When removing from the analysis a study that from visual inspection of the graph appears to be an outlier, the I2 statistic goes down to 0% (analysis not shown). This study reported an effect estimate in the direction favouring placebo over galantamine after 12 weeks treatment (RR 3.00, 95% CI 0.96 to 9.39; 38 participants, 1 RCT, Analysis 1.14); the other studies reported effect estimates in the direction favouring lecithin or deanol over placebo after 11 days to eight weeks treatment (see Analysis 1.14).

1.8 Sensitivity analyses
1.8.1 Implication of randomisation

We aimed to include trials in a sensitivity analysis if they were described in some way as to imply randomisation. As all studies were stated to be randomised we have not undertaken this sensitivity analysis.

1.8.2 Assumptions for lost binary data

We would have undertaken a sensitivity analysis assessing where assumptions had to be made regarding people lost to follow‐up for the primary outcome (see Dealing with missing data). We intended to compare the findings when we used our assumption compared with completer data only. No assumptions had to be made, studies that reported data for the primary outcome reported for all randomised participants.

1.8.3 Risk of bias

When excluding three trials from the primary outcome that we judged to be at high risk of bias across one or more of the domains, there was no substantial alteration to the direction of effect or the precision of the effect estimates (RR 1.00, 95% CI 0.43 to 2.34; 1 study, 5 people, analysis not shown).

1.8.4 Imputed values

We would have undertaken a sensitivity analysis to assess the effects of including data from cluster‐randomised trials where we used imputed values for ICC in calculating the design effect. No cluster‐randomised trials were included.

1.8.5 Fixed and random effects

We also synthesised data for the primary outcome using a random‐effects model. This did not alter the significance of the results (RR 0.94, 95% CI 0.71 to 1.25; 4 studies, 27 people, analysis not shown).

2. Comparison 2. Cholinergic drugs versus other cholinergic drugs

Only one study (George 1981) was identified that reported on this comparison. When comparing a higher dose of deanol (2000 mg/day) to a lower dose (1000 mg/day) there was a slight improvement in TD scores for the group with the higher dose (1 trial, 22 people, RR 0.50, 95% CI 0.25 to 0.98, Analysis 2.1). No events were reported between the different doses for death or leaving the study early (Analysis 2.2; Analysis 2.3).

We did not identify any studies that reported on adverse events, mental state, hospital and service utilisation outcomes, economic outcomes, social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, personalised quality of life, behaviour, or cognitive state.

Discussion

Summary of main results

1. The search

This 2017 update has identified additional data from three studies (Caroff 2007; Jahanian 2014; Ogunmefun 2009) of new CNS‐active cholinergic agents mainly used for the treatment of Alzheimer's disease (donezepil, galantamine, and rivastigmine). It is probable, however, that the effect of these modern cholinergic agents for TD has not been comprehensively investigated within randomised trials. This update did not identify any new trials of the old cholinergic drugs (deanol, lecithin, meclofenoxate); trials of these drugs were published from 1976 to 1990.

2. Few data

Only a little over 350 people have been included in this review. It is possible that real, and important, effects have not been highlighted because of the necessarily wide CIs of the findings. Many outcomes were not measured at all (see Overall completeness and applicability of evidence), including one of our pre‐stated outcome measures. We may have been overambitious in hoping for some of these outcomes in TD trials, but simple reporting of quality of life still does not seem too demanding and does remain of interest.

3. Comparison 1. Cholinergic drugs versus placebo

3.1 TD symptoms

We are uncertain about the evidence on no clinically important improvement in TD symptoms comparing cholinergic drugs and placebo as the evidence was of very low quality (RR 0.89, 95% CI 0.65 to 1.23, 4 trials, 27 people). There may be little or no difference in deterioration of TD symptoms between cholinergic drugs and placebo, evidence was of low quality (RR 1.11, 95% CI 0.55 to 2.24, 8 trials, 147 people).

3.2 Mental state

We are uncertain about the evidence on deterioration of mental state comparing cholinergic drugs and placebo as the evidence was of very low quality (RR 0.50, 95% CI 0.10 to 2.61, 77 people, 5 trials).

3.3 Adverse events

We are uncertain about the evidence on adverse events comparing cholinergic drugs and placebo as the evidence was of very low quality (RR 0.56, 95% CI 0.15 to 2.14, 106 people, 4 trials).

3.4 Acceptability of treatment

It is always unclear what leaving the study early means. It could be to do with the participant not accepting treatment for a series of reasons, or of participants finding the trial intolerable. It also could be a function of a trial design in which willing participants are still asked to leave because of some degree of protocol violation. In any event, four studies reported that 14/53 people left the cholinergics group compared with a not significantly different 12/50 people in the placebo group. Eight studies reported that no adverse events occurred.

3.5 Social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life

This group of outcomes was selected as being of importance to patients for the 2017 review update following a service user consultation. None of the included studies reported on this outcome.

See summary of findings Table for the main comparison for a summary of the evidence.

4. Cholinergic drugs versus other cholinergic drugs

One study (George 1981) included study arms investigating two doses of deanol, 1 g and 2 g per dose, and found that twice as many participants experienced any improvement in TD symptoms with the higher dose (RR 0.50, 95% CI 0.25 to 0.98, 22 people). Very few participants were randomised, so the evidence is uncertain. Mental state, adverse events, or social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life were not reported. No events were reported for leaving the study early.

Overall completeness and applicability of evidence

1. Completeness

The most important finding of this review is that a systematic search of the literature still results in a review that is considerably underpowered to really investigate the clinical efficacy and safety of cholinergic agents in TD. All data are inconclusive. We were unable to detect any effect, good or bad of these drugs for TD. With the advent of new CNS‐active cholinergic agents for treatment of Alzheimer's disease and dementia (e.g., Birks 2006a; Birks 2006b; Birks 2015; Li 2015; Loy 2006; Maidment 2006; Rolinski 2012), the theoretical base for this review is strengthened. However, the actual central cholinergic transmission ‐enhancing effect of old cholinergic drugs, such as lecithin and especially deanol, remains unclear. Even if any of these old compounds had an effect on cholinergic transmission, the impact would probably be limited. Modern cholinergic drugs use a different mode of action and are worthy of investigation, although they are associated with gastrointestinal adverse events (Birks 2006a; Birks 2006b; Birks 2015; Li 2015; Loy 2006; Maidment 2006; Rolinski 2012). However, to date, these newer drugs have not been fully investigated in RCTs in the treatment of TD.

2. Applicability

All but two trials were hospital‐based but were nevertheless on people who would be recognisable in everyday care. Trials were set in high‐income countries in Asia, Australia, Europe and North America. Cholinergic drugs are readily accessible and most outcomes understandable in terms of clinical practice. Should cholinergic drugs have had important effects the findings may well have been applicable.

Quality of the evidence

Overall, the certainty of the evidence in this review is low to very low. This means that we have very little confidence in the effect estimates, and the true effects are likely to be substantially different from the estimates of the effect. The following are the main reasons for our low confidence in the evidence.

  1. Poor study methodology and reporting of methods and data (see Figure 3 and Figure 4) resulting in downgrading evidence for risk of bias. Allocation concealment was not described, generation of the sequence was not explicit, studies were not clearly blinded, we are unsure if data are incomplete, and data were often poorly or selectively reported.

  2. Very small sample sizes resulting in downgrading evidence for imprecision. The largest trial in this review randomised only 24 people. A trial of this size is unable to detect subtle, yet important differences due to cholinergic drugs with any confidence. In order to detect a 20% difference between groups, probably about 150 people are needed in each arm of the study (alpha 0.05, beta 0.8).

  3. Wide CIs (often due to low event rates) that included appreciable benefit or harm for the intervention as well as no effect, resulting in downgrading evidence for imprecision.

Please see summary of findings Table for the main comparison for full details.

Potential biases in the review process

1. Missing studies

Every effort was made to identify relevant trials. However, these studies are all small and it is likely that we have failed to identify other studies of limited power. It is likely that such studies would also not be in favour of the cholinergics group. If they had been so, it is more likely that they would have been published in accessible literature. We do not, however, think it likely that we have failed to identify large relevant studies.

2. Introducing bias

We have tried to be balanced in our appraisal of the evidence but could have inadvertently introduced bias. We welcome comments or criticisms. New methods and innovations now make it possible to report data where, in the past, we could not report data at all or had to report data in a different way. We believe that 'Summary of findings' tables are a valuable innovation – but problematic to those not ‘blind’ to the outcome data. It is possible to ‘cherry pick’ significant findings for presentation in this table. We have tried to decrease the chance of doing this by asking a new review author (HB) to select outcomes relevant for this table before becoming familiar with the data.

Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews

The only other relevant quantitative reviews we know of is the previous Cochrane review (Tammenmaa 2002) and another systematic review that included the same RCTs as the Cochrane review (Tammenmaa 2004). These reviews found no evidence to support administration of the old cholinergic agents lecithin, deanol, and meclofenoxate to patients with TD, and concluded that further investigation of the clinical effects of novel cholinergic agents in TD is warranted. This update identified three new studies to include (as discussed in Results of the search), but does not substantially change these findings or conclusions.

Message from one of the participants of the Public and patient involvement consultation of service user perspectives on tardive dyskinesia research.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 1

Message from one of the participants of the Public and patient involvement consultation of service user perspectives on tardive dyskinesia research.

Study flow diagram for 2015 and 2017 searches.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 2

Study flow diagram for 2015 and 2017 searches.

'Risk of bias, summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 3

'Risk of bias, summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 4

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 1 Tardive dyskinesia: 1. No clinically important improvement (50% or more change on any validated TD scale).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.1

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 1 Tardive dyskinesia: 1. No clinically important improvement (50% or more change on any validated TD scale).

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 2 Tardive dyskinesia: 2a. Not any improvement (as assessed by rater).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.2

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 2 Tardive dyskinesia: 2a. Not any improvement (as assessed by rater).

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 3 Tardive dyskinesia: 2b. Not any improvement (as assessed by self report).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.3

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 3 Tardive dyskinesia: 2b. Not any improvement (as assessed by self report).

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 4 Tardive dyskinesia: 3a. Average endpoint score on AIMS (low score = better).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.4

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 4 Tardive dyskinesia: 3a. Average endpoint score on AIMS (low score = better).

Study

Intervention

Mean

SD

N

Comments

deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

Tarsy 1977

Deanol

10

5.48

4

Tarsy 1977

Placebo

10

0

1

The confidence interval of mean difference was not estimable because the placebo group only had one participant.

Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.5

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 5 Tardive dyskinesia: 3b. Average endpoint score on modified Simpson TDRS (low score = better).

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 6 Tardive dyskinesia: 4a. Deterioration (as assessed by rater).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.6

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 6 Tardive dyskinesia: 4a. Deterioration (as assessed by rater).

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 7 Tardive dyskinesia: 4b. Deterioration (as assessed by self report).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.7

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 7 Tardive dyskinesia: 4b. Deterioration (as assessed by self report).

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 8 Global outcome: Death for any reason.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.8

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 8 Global outcome: Death for any reason.

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 9 Global outcome: Intervention not useful as assessed by Global Usefulness Rating (GUR).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.9

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 9 Global outcome: Intervention not useful as assessed by Global Usefulness Rating (GUR).

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 10 Global state: Average endpoint score on CGI (low score = better).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.10

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 10 Global state: Average endpoint score on CGI (low score = better).

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 11 Mental state: Deterioration.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.11

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 11 Mental state: Deterioration.

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 12 Adverse effects: Any.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.12

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 12 Adverse effects: Any.

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 13 Adverse effects: Various specific.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.13

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 13 Adverse effects: Various specific.

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 14 Leaving the study early.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.14

Comparison 1 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO, Outcome 14 Leaving the study early.

Comparison 2 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus OTHER CHOLINERGIC DRUGS, Outcome 1 Tardive dyskinesia: 2a. Not any improvement (as assessed by rater) ‐ less than 6 weeks.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 2.1

Comparison 2 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus OTHER CHOLINERGIC DRUGS, Outcome 1 Tardive dyskinesia: 2a. Not any improvement (as assessed by rater) ‐ less than 6 weeks.

Comparison 2 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus OTHER CHOLINERGIC DRUGS, Outcome 2 Global outcome: Death for any reason ‐ less than 6 weeks.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 2.2

Comparison 2 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus OTHER CHOLINERGIC DRUGS, Outcome 2 Global outcome: Death for any reason ‐ less than 6 weeks.

Comparison 2 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus OTHER CHOLINERGIC DRUGS, Outcome 3 Leaving the study early ‐ less than 6 weeks.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 2.3

Comparison 2 CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus OTHER CHOLINERGIC DRUGS, Outcome 3 Leaving the study early ‐ less than 6 weeks.

Table 2. Excluded studies which are relevant to other reviews

Excluded study

Comparison

Treatment category

Relevant review

#1

#2

Nasrallah 1986

Alpha‐methyl‐p‐tyrosine (AMPT)

versus choline chloride

Amino acid

Organic salt

versus hydroxytryptophan

Amino acid (serotonin precursor)

versus valproic acid

Mood stabilisers

versus L‐DOPA

Amino acid

Lieberman 1988

Benztropine versus bromocriptine

Anticholinergic

Dopamine agonist

Bromocriptine versus haloperidol

Dopamine agonist

Antipsychotic

Nasrallah 1986

Choline chloride

versus L‐DOPA

Organic salt

Amino acid

versus hydroxytryptophan

versus valproic

Anticonvulsant

Jus 1978

Deanol

versus lithium carbonate

Antidepressant

Organic salt

versus placebo

Placebo

Chien 1978

versus sodium valproate

Anticonvulsant

versus oxpertine

Antipsychotic

Nasrallah 1986

Hydroxytryptophan versus L‐DOPA

Amino acid

Amino acid

L‐DOPA versus valproic acid

Anticonvulsant

Jus 1978

Lithium carbonate versus placebo

Mood stabiliser

Placebo

Chien 1978

Oxypertine versus sodium valproate

Antipsychotic

Anticonvulsant

Figuras y tablas -
Table 2. Excluded studies which are relevant to other reviews
Table 3. Suggestions for design of future study

Methods

Allocation: randomised, with sequence generation and concealment of allocation clearly described.
Blindness: double, tested.
Duration: 12 months beyond end of intervention at least.
Raters: independent.

Participants

People with antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia.*
Age: any.
Sex: both.
History: any.
N = 300.**

Interventions

Specific cholinergic drug (N = 150) versus placebo (N = 150)

Outcomes

Tardive dyskinesia: any clinically important improvement in TD, any improvement, deterioration.***
Adverse effects: no clinically significant extrapyramidal adverse effects ‐ any time period***, use of any antiparkinsonism drugs, other important adverse events.
Leaving the study early.
Service outcomes: admitted, number of admissions, length of hospitalisation, contacts with psychiatric services.
Compliance with drugs.
Economic evaluations: cost‐effectiveness, cost‐benefit.
General state: relapse, frequency and intensity of minor and major exacerbations.
Social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life: binary measure
Distress among relatives: binary measure.
Burden on family: binary measure.

Notes

* This could be diagnosed by clinical decision. If funds were permitting all participants could be screened using operational criteria, otherwise a random sample should suffice.

** Size of study with sufficient power to highlight about a 10% difference between groups for primary outcome.
*** Primary outcome. The same applies to the measure of primary outcome as for diagnosis. Not everyone may need to have operational criteria applied if clinical impression is proved to be accurate.

Figuras y tablas -
Table 3. Suggestions for design of future study
Summary of findings for the main comparison. CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO for antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia

CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO for antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia

Patient or population: people with various psychiatric disorders (mainly schizophrenia) and antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia
Settings: mostly inpatients in Australia, Canada, Germany, Japan, Switzerland and the USA.
Intervention: CHOLINERGIC DRUGS (deanol, donepezil, galantamine, meclofenoxate, lecithin) versus PLACEBO

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of Participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

PLACEBO

CHOLINERGIC DRUGS

Tardive dyskinesia: No clinically important improvement

follow‐up: 2 to 12 weeks

1000 per 1000

890 per 1000
(650 to 1000)

RR 0.89
(0.65 to 1.23)

27
(4 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (deanol, donepezil, lecithin) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Tardive dyskinesia: Deterioration

follow‐up: 9 days to 12 weeks

116 per 1000

129 per 1000
(64 to 260)

RR 1.11
(0.55 to 2.24)

147
(8 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
low1,3

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (deanol, donepezil, lecithin, meclofenoxate) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Mental state: Deterioration

follow‐up: 11 days to 12 weeks

56 per 1000

28 per 1000
(6 to 145)

RR 0.50
(0.10 to 2.61)

77
(5 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (deanol, lecithin) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Adverse effects: Any adverse events

follow‐up: 9 days to 8 weeks

98 per 1000

55 per 1000
(15 to 210)

RR 0.56
(0.15 to 2.14)

106
(4 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (donepezil, lecithin, meclofenoxate) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Acceptability of treatment: Leaving the study early

follow‐up: 9 days to 12 weeks

90 per 1000

98 per 1000
(50 to 188)

RR 1.09
(0.56 to 2.10)

288
(12 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,3,4

None of the subgroups that reported on this outcome (deanol, donepezil, galantamine, meclofenoxate, lecithin) found a significant difference between cholinergic drug and placebo.

Social confidence, social inclusion, social networks, or personalised quality of life ‐ not reported

None of the included studies reported on this outcome.

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1 Downgraded one step for risk of bias: for many studies it was unclear whether randomisation procedure and allocation concealment were carried out adequately.
2 Downgraded two steps for imprecision: very few events and participants, wide CIs that include both no effect and appreciable benefit for the intervention.
3 Downgraded one step for imprecision: wide CIs that include appreciable benefit for both the intervention and the control group, as well as no effect.
4 Downgraded one step for indirectness: leaving the study early is not a direct measure of acceptability of the intervention.

Figuras y tablas -
Summary of findings for the main comparison. CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO for antipsychotic‐induced tardive dyskinesia
Table 1. Other reviews in the series

Interventions

Reference

Anticholinergic medication

Soares‐Weiser 1997

Benzodiazepines

Bhoopathi 2006

Calcium channel blockers

Essali 2011

Cholinergic medication

This review

Gamma‐aminobutyric acid agonists

Alabed 2011

Miscellaneous treatments

Soares‐Weiser 2003

Neuroleptic reduction and/or cessation and neuroleptics

Soares‐Weiser 2006

Non‐neuroleptic catecholaminergic drugs

El‐Sayeh 2006

Vitamin E

Soares‐Weiser 2011

Figuras y tablas -
Table 1. Other reviews in the series
Comparison 1. CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Tardive dyskinesia: 1. No clinically important improvement (50% or more change on any validated TD scale) Show forest plot

4

27

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.89 [0.65, 1.23]

1.1 deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

2

11

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.91 [0.51, 1.60]

1.2 donepezil ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

10

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.0 [0.70, 1.43]

1.3 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

6

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.71 [0.31, 1.66]

2 Tardive dyskinesia: 2a. Not any improvement (as assessed by rater) Show forest plot

9

180

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.87 [0.71, 1.07]

2.1 deanol ‐ less than 6 weeks

3

63

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.83 [0.58, 1.18]

2.2 deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

2

11

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.81 [0.26, 2.57]

2.3 donepezil ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

10

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.33 [0.72, 2.44]

2.4 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

2

36

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.87 [0.63, 1.21]

2.5 meclofenoxate ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.84 [0.55, 1.27]

3 Tardive dyskinesia: 2b. Not any improvement (as assessed by self report) Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

3.1 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

30

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.92 [0.62, 1.36]

4 Tardive dyskinesia: 3a. Average endpoint score on AIMS (low score = better) Show forest plot

7

171

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

‐0.12 [‐0.44, 0.21]

4.1 deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

6

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.42 [‐0.29, 3.13]

4.2 galantamine ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

35

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.5 [‐0.44, 3.44]

4.3 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

6

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

‐1.07 [‐2.21, 0.07]

4.4 lecithin ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

14

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

‐0.10 [‐1.04, 0.84]

4.5 meclofenoxate ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

60

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

‐0.19 [‐0.58, 0.20]

4.6 rivastigmine ‐ less than 8 weeks

1

40

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.20 [‐1.16, 5.56]

4.7 donepezil ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

10

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.10 [‐4.22, 6.42]

5 Tardive dyskinesia: 3b. Average endpoint score on modified Simpson TDRS (low score = better) Show forest plot

Other data

No numeric data

5.1 deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

Other data

No numeric data

6 Tardive dyskinesia: 4a. Deterioration (as assessed by rater) Show forest plot

8

147

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.11 [0.55, 2.24]

6.1 deanol ‐ less than 6 weeks

2

30

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.67 [0.48, 5.76]

6.2 deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

2

11

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.67 [0.20, 2.18]

6.3 donepezil ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

10

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.67 [0.06, 7.85]

6.4 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

2

36

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.0 [0.16, 6.31]

6.5 meclofenoxate ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.87 [0.18, 19.55]

7 Tardive dyskinesia: 4b. Deterioration (as assessed by self report) Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

7.1 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

30

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

3.0 [0.13, 68.26]

8 Global outcome: Death for any reason Show forest plot

11

278

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

8.1 deanol ‐ less than 6 weeks

4

83

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

8.2 deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

2

11

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

8.3 galantamine ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

38

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

8.4 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

3

86

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

8.5 meclofenoxate ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

9 Global outcome: Intervention not useful as assessed by Global Usefulness Rating (GUR) Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

9.1 meclofenoxate ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.89 [0.59, 1.32]

10 Global state: Average endpoint score on CGI (low score = better) Show forest plot

1

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

10.1 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

31

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

‐0.43 [‐1.36, 0.50]

11 Mental state: Deterioration Show forest plot

5

77

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.5 [0.10, 2.61]

11.1 deanol ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

10

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.33 [0.02, 6.65]

11.2 deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

2

11

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.2 [0.08, 18.75]

11.3 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

2

56

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.33 [0.01, 7.81]

12 Adverse effects: Any Show forest plot

4

106

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.56 [0.15, 2.14]

12.1 donepezil ‐ less than 6 weeks ‐ any adverse events

1

10

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

12.2 lecithin ‐ more than 6 weeks ‐ any other adverse effects, undesirable body odour, sedation

2

36

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

12.3 meclofenoxate ‐ more than 6 weeks ‐ any adverse events

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.56 [0.15, 2.14]

13 Adverse effects: Various specific Show forest plot

8

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

13.1 deanol ‐ less than 6 weeks ‐ gastric adverse effects

5

61

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

9.0 [0.55, 147.95]

13.2 deanol ‐ less than 6 weeks ‐ sedation, periferal cholinergic effects, undesirable body odour

6

94

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

6.83 [0.99, 47.25]

13.3 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks ‐ GI adverse effects

2

36

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

14 Leaving the study early Show forest plot

12

288

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.09 [0.56, 2.10]

14.1 deanol ‐ less than 6 weeks

4

83

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.33 [0.02, 6.65]

14.2 deanol ‐ more than 6 weeks

2

11

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.2 [0.08, 18.75]

14.3 donepezil ‐ less than 6 weeks

1

10

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

14.4 galantamine ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

38

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

3.0 [0.96, 9.39]

14.5 lecithin ‐ less than 6 weeks

3

86

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.5 [0.17, 1.45]

14.6 meclofenoxate ‐ more than 6 weeks

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

Figuras y tablas -
Comparison 1. CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus PLACEBO
Comparison 2. CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus OTHER CHOLINERGIC DRUGS

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Tardive dyskinesia: 2a. Not any improvement (as assessed by rater) ‐ less than 6 weeks Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

1.1 deanol 2g vs deanol 1g

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

2 Global outcome: Death for any reason ‐ less than 6 weeks Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

2.1 deanol 2g vs deanol 1g

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

3 Leaving the study early ‐ less than 6 weeks Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

3.1 deanol 2g vs deanol 1g

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

Figuras y tablas -
Comparison 2. CHOLINERGIC DRUGS versus OTHER CHOLINERGIC DRUGS