Scolaris Content Display Scolaris Content Display

مقیاس ارزیابی رفتاری نوزادان (NBAS) و سیستم مشاهدات رفتاری نوزاد تازه متولد شده (NBO) در حمایت از مراقبان و بهبود پیامدها در مراقبان و نوزادان آنها

Contraer todo Desplegar todo

Referencias

Anderson 1981 {published data only}

Anderson CJ. Enhancing reciprocity between mother and neonate. Nursing Research1981; Vol. 30, issue 2:89‐93. [PUBMED: 6907868]CENTRAL

Beal 1989 {published data only}

Beal JA. The effect on father infant interaction of demonstrating the neonatal behavioural assessment scale. Birth1989; Vol. 16, issue 1:18‐22. [PUBMED: 2742633]CENTRAL

Beeghly 1995a {published data only}

Beeghly M, Brazelton TB, Flannery KA, Nugent JK, Barrett DE, Tronick EZ. Specificity of preventative pediatric intervention effects in early infancy. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics1995; Vol. 16, issue 3:158‐66. [PUBMED: 7560118]CENTRAL

Belsky 1985 {published data only}

Belsky J. Experimenting with the family in the newborn period. Child Development1985; Vol. 56:407‐14. [PUBMED: 3987415]CENTRAL

Britt 1994 {published data only}

Britt GC, Myers BJ. Testing the effectiveness of BNAS intervention with a substance‐using population. Infant Mental Health Journal 1994;15(3):293‐304. [DOI: 10.1002/1097‐0355(199423)15:3<293::AID‐IMHJ2280150305>3.0.CO;2‐E]CENTRAL

Furr 1982 {published data only}

Furr PA, Kirgis CA. A nurse‐midwifery approach to early mother‐infant acquaintance. Journal of Midwifery & Women's Health 1982;27(5):10‐4. [DOI: 10.1016/0091‐2182(82)90004‐0; PUBMED: 6922161]CENTRAL

Killough 2004 {published data only}

Killough RH. The Birth of Parenting: Maternal Representations, the Place of the Infant, and the Development of Sensitive Caring [PhD thesis]. Amherst: University of Massachusetts Amherst, 2004. CENTRAL

Liptak 1983 {published data only}

Liptak GS, Keller BB, Feldman AW, Chamberlin RW. Enhancing infant development and parent‐practitioner interaction with the Brazelton Neonatal Assessment Scale. Pediatrics1983; Vol. 72, issue 1:71‐8. [PUBMED: 6866594]CENTRAL

McManus 2012 {published data only}

McManus BM, Nugent JK. A neurobehavioral intervention incorporated into a state early intervention program is associated with higher perceived quality of care among parents of high‐risk newborns. Journal of Behavioral Health Services and Research. 2012/04/25 2014; Vol. 41, issue 3:381‐9. [DOI: 10.1007/s11414‐012‐9283‐1; PUBMED: 22529036]CENTRAL

Myers 1982a {published data only}

Myers BJ. Early interventions using Brazelton training with middle‐class mothers and fathers of newborns. Child Development1982; Vol. 53, issue 2:462‐71. [DOI: 10.2307/1128989]CENTRAL

Nugent 2014 {published data only}

Nugent JK, Bartlett JD, Valim, C. Effects of an infant‐focused relationship‐based hospital and home visiting intervention on reducing symptoms of postpartum maternal depression: a pilot study. Infants and Young Children2014; Vol. 27, issue 4:292‐304. [DOI: 10.1097/IYC.0000000000000017; 0896‐3746]CENTRAL

Nugent 2015 {published data only}

Nugent JK, Dym‐Bartlett J, Von Ende A, Killough J, Valim C. A randomized study of the effects of the Newborn Behavioural Observations (NBO) system on sensitivity in mother‐infant interaction. Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting; 2015 Mar 19‐21; Philadelphia (PA). 2015. CENTRAL

Pannabecker 1982 {published data only}

Pannabecker BJ, Emde RN, Austin BC. The effect of early extended contact on father‐newborn interaction. The Journal of Genetic Psychology1982; Vol. 141, issue 1st Half:7‐17. [DOI: 10.1080/00221325.1982.10533452; PUBMED: 7142984 ]CENTRAL

Poley‐Strobel 1987 {published data only}

Poley‐Strobel BA, Beckmann, CA. The effects of a teaching‐modeling intervention on early mother‐infant reciprocity. Infant Behavior and Development1987; Vol. 10, issue 4:467‐76. [DOI: 10.1016/0163‐6383(87)90043‐9]CENTRAL

Szajnberg 1987 {published data only}

Szajnberg N, Ward MJ, Krauss A, Kessler DB. Low birth‐weight prematures: preventive intervention and maternal attitude. Child Psychiatry and Human Development1987; Vol. 17, issue 3:152‐65. [DOI: 10.1007/BF00706226]CENTRAL

Worobey 1982 {published data only}

Worobey J, Belsky J. Employing the Brazelton scale to influence mothering: an experimental comparison of three strategies. Developmental Psychology1982; Vol. 18, issue 5:736‐43. [DOI: 10.1037/0012‐1649.18.5.736]CENTRAL

Cardone 1990 {published data only}

Cardone IA, Gilkerson L. Family administered neonatal activities: an exploratory method for the integration of parental perceptions and newborn behaviour. Infant Mental Health Journal 1990;11(2):127‐41. [DOI: 10.1002/1097‐0355(199022)11:2<127::AID‐IMHJ2280110205>3.0.CO;2‐A]CENTRAL

Cooper 2009 {published data only}

Cooper PJ, Tomlinson M, Swartz L, Landman M, Molteno C, Stein A, et al. Improving quality of mother‐infant relationship and infant attachment in socioeconomically deprived community in South Africa: randomised controlled trial. BMJ 2009;338:b1858. [DOI: 10.1136/bmj.b974]CENTRAL

Cooper 2015 {published data only}

Cooper PJ, De Pascalis L, Woolgar M, Romaniuk H, Murray L. Attempting to prevent postnatal depression by targeting the mother‐infant relationship: a randomised controlled trial. Primary Health Care Research and Development 2015;16(4):383‐97. [DOI: 10.1017/S1463423614000401; PUBMED: 25381790]CENTRAL

Egeland 1984 {published data only}

Egeland B, Farber EA. Infant‐mother attachment: factors related to its development and changes over time. Child Development 1984;55(3):753‐71. [PUBMED: 6734316]CENTRAL

Fowles 1999 {published data only}

Fowles ER. The Brazelton Neonatal Behavioural Assessment Scale and maternal identity. American Journal of Maternal Child Nursing 1999;24(6):287‐93. [PUBMED: 10565142]CENTRAL

Golas 1986 {published data only}

Golas GA, Parks P. Effect of early postpartum teaching on primiparas' knowledge of infant behavior and degree of confidence. Research in Nursing and Health 1986;9(3):209‐14. [DOI: 10.1002/nur.4770090305]CENTRAL

Gomes 1987 {published data only}

Gomes‐Pedro J. Early Intervention and Mother‐Infant Interaction During the First Three Months of Life. Washington DC: ERIC Clearinghouse, 1987. CENTRAL

Gomespedro 1995 {published data only}

Gomes‐Pedro J, Patricio M, Carvalho A, Goldschmidt T, Torqal‐Garcia F, Monteiro MB. Early intervention with Portuguese mothers: a two year follow up. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 1995;16(1):21‐8. [PUBMED: 7730453]CENTRAL

Hart 1998 {published data only}

Hart S, Field T, Nearing G. Depressed mothers' neonates improve following the MABI and a Brazelton demonstration. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 1998;23(6):351‐6. [DOI: 10.1093/jpepsy/23.6.351]CENTRAL

Joshi 2013 {published data only}

Joshi K, Puntambekar A. Effects of early intervention on the behaviour of premature neonates ‐ a clinical trial. Journal of Physical Therapy 2013;8(1):12‐7. CENTRAL

Kiepura 2011 {published data only}

Kiepura E, Kmita G, Cieślak‐Osik B, Urmańska W, Lewandowska D. Neonatal behavioural assessment of pre‐term and full‐term infants as experiences by parents [Ocena zachowania noworodka w doświadczeniu rodziców dzieci urodzonych przedwcześnie i o czasie]. Medycyna Wieku Rozwojowego 2011;15(3 Pt 2):414‐20. [PUBMED: 22253127]CENTRAL

Kusaka 2007 {published data only}

Kusaka R, Ohgi S, Gima H, Fujimoto T. Short term effects of the neonatal behavioural assessment scale based intervention for infants with developmental disabilities. Journal of Physical Therapy Science 2007;19(1):1‐8. [DOI: 10.1589/jpts.19.1]CENTRAL

Meisels 1993 {published data only}

Meisels S, Dichtelmiller M, Liaw FR. A multidimensional analysis of early childhood intervention programme. In: Zeanah CH editor(s). Handbook of Infant Mental Health. 3rd Edition. Vol. 1, Guilford Press, 1993:361‐442. CENTRAL

Nurcombe 1984 {published data only}

Nurcombe B, Howell DC, Rauh VA, Teti DM, Ruoff P, Brennan J. An intervention program for mothers of low‐birthweight infants: preliminary results. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1984;23(3):319‐25. [DOI: 10.1016/S0002‐7138(09)60511‐2]CENTRAL

Ogi 2001 {published data only}

Ogi S, Arisawa K, Takahashi T, Akiyama T, Goto Y, Fukuda M, et al. The developmental effects of an early intervention programme for very low birthweight infants. Brain and Development 2001;33(1):31‐6. [PUBMED: 11197893]CENTRAL

Ohgi 2004 {published data only}

Ohgi S, Fukuda M, Akiyama T, Gima H. Effect of an early intervention programme on low birthweight infants with cerebral injuries. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health 2004;40(12):689‐95. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1440‐1754.2004.00512.x; PUBMED: 15569286 ]CENTRAL

Parker 1992 {published data only}

Parker S, Zahr LK, Cole JG, Brecht ML. Outcome after developmental intervention in the neonatal intensive care unit for mothers of preterm infants with low socioeconomic status. Journal of Paediatrics 1992;120(5):780‐5. [PUBMED: 1578316]CENTRAL

Parker Loewen 1987 {published data only}

Parker‐Loewen DL, Lytton H. Effects of short‐term interaction coaching with mothers of preterm infants. Infant Mental Health Journal 1987;8(3):277‐87. [DOI: 10.1002/1097‐0355(198723)8:3<277::AID‐IMHJ2280080310>3.0.CO;2‐X]CENTRAL

Sanders 2006 {published data only}

Sanders LW, Buckner EB. The Newborn Behavioral Observations system as a nursing intervention to enhance engagement in first‐time mothers: feasibility and desirability. Pediatric Nursing 2006;32(5):455‐9. [PUBMED: 17100077]CENTRAL

Sullivan 1980 {published data only}

Sullivan DL, Leake RD. Significant effect of the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioural Assessment Scale in maternal training. Pediatric Research 1980;14(4):439. CENTRAL

Teti 2009 {published data only}

Teti DM, Black MM, Viscardi R, Glass P, O'Connell MA, Baker L, et al. Intervention with African American premature infants: four months results of an early intervention program. Journal of Early Intervention 2009;31(2):146‐66. [DOI: 10.1177/1053815109331864]CENTRAL

Wendland Carro 1999 {published data only}

Wendland‐Carro J, Piccinini CA, Millar WS. The role of an early intervention on enhancing the quality of mother‐infant interaction. Child Development 1999;70(3):713‐21. [PUBMED: 10368917]CENTRAL

Widmayer 1980 {published data only}

Widmayer SM, Field TM. Effects of Brazelton demonstrations on early interactions of preterm infants and their teenage mothers. Infant Behaviour and Development 1980;3(1):79‐89. [DOI: 10.1016/S0163‐6383(80)80008‐7]CENTRAL

Widmayer 1981 {published data only}

Widmayer SM, Field TM. Effects of Brazelton demonstrations for mother on the development of preterm infants. Pediatrics 1981;67(5):711‐4. [PUBMED: 7255001]CENTRAL

NCT03070652 {unpublished data only}

NCT03070652. What are the effects of supporting early parenting by increasing the understanding of the infant? [What are the effects of supporting early parenting by increasing the understanding of the infant? A randomized community based trial]. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03070652 (first received 3 March 2017). CENTRAL

Nicolson 2016 {unpublished data only}

Nicolson S. The UNA Study. Understanding your newborn and adapting to parenthood. Centre for Women's Mental Health. Unpublished manuscript2016. CENTRAL

Nugent 2016 {unpublished data only}

Nugent JK, McManus B. The effects of the NBO early intervention model of care on infants with developmental disabilities on infant self‐regulation, parent‐infant interaction and maternal depression and later social‐emotional functioning. Unpublished manuscript2016. CENTRAL

Abidin 1983

Abidin RR. The Parenting Stress Index. Charlottesville (VA): Pediatric Psychology Press, 1983.

Ainsworth 1971

Ainsworth MDS, Bell SM, Stayton DJ. Individual differences in strange situation behavior of one‐year‐olds. In: Schaffer HR editor(s). The Origins of Human Social Relations. New York (NY): Academic Press, 1971:17‐57.

Ainsworth 1974

Ainsworth MDS, Bell SM, Stayton DJ. Infant‐mother attachment and social development: socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In: Richards MPM editor(s). The Integration of a Child into a Social World. London: Cambridge University Press, 1974:97‐119.

Barett 1988

Barett, D, Beeghly, M, Burrows, E, Brazelton TB. Effects of IUGR. Maternal Characteristics and Intervention on Maternal Behaviour with the 4‐month old infant. Infant Association for Infant Mental Health. Providence: RI, 1988.

Barnard 1978

Barnard K. Nursing Child Assessment Teaching Scale. Seattle (WA): University of Washington, 1978.

Bates 1979

Bates JE, Bennett Freeland CA, Lounsbury ML. Measurement of infant difficultness. Child Development 1979;50(3):794‐803. [DOI: 10.2307/1128946]

Bayley 1969

Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development: Birth to Two Years. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1969.

Beebe 2010

Beebe B, Jaffe J, Markese S, Buck K, Chen H, Cohen P, et al. The origins of 12‐month attachment: a microanalysis of 4‐month mother‐infant interaction. Attachment and Human Development 2010;12(1‐2):3‐141. [DOI: 10.1080/14616730903338985; PUBMED: 20390524]

Beebe 2012

Beebe B, Lachmann F, Jaffe J, Markese S, Buck KA, Chen H, et al. Maternal postpartum depressive symptoms and 4‐month mother‐infant interaction. Psychoanalytic Psychology 2012;29(4):383‐407. [DOI: 10.1037/a0029387]

Beeghly 1995b

Beeghly M, Brazelton TB, Flannery KA, Nugent JK, Barrett DE, Tronick EZ. Specificity of preventative pediatric intervention effects in early infancy. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 1995;16(3):158‐66. [PUBMED: 7560118]

Belsky 1984

Belsky J, Rovine M, Taylor DG. The Pennsylvania Infant and Family Development Project, III: the origins of individual differences in infant‐mother attachment: maternal and infant contributions. Child Development 1984;55(3):718‐28. [DOI: 10.2307/1130124]

Belsky 1996

Belsky J, Campbell SB, Cohn JF, Moore G. Instability of infant‐parent attachment security. Developmental Psychology 1996;32(5):921‐4. [DOI: 10.1037//0012‐1649.32.5.921]

Belsky 2002

Belsky J, Fearon RM. Infant‐mother attachment security, contextual risk, and early development: a moderational analysis. Development and Psychopathology 2002;14(2):293‐310. [PUBMED: 12030693]

Berlin 2008

Berlin LJ, Cassidy J, Appleyard KC, Shaver PR. The influence of early attachments on other relationships. Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. 2nd Edition. New York (US): Guilford Press, 2008:333‐47.

Borelli 2010

Borelli JL, David DH, Crowley MJ, Mayes LC. Links between disorganized attachment classification and clinical symptoms in school‐aged children. Journal of Child and Family Studies 2010;19(3):243‐56. [DOI: 10.1007/s10826‐009‐9292‐8]

Bornstein 2013

Bornstein MH, Manian N. Maternal responsiveness and sensitivity re‐considered: some is more. Development and Psychopathology 2013;25(4 Pt 1):957‐71. [DOI: 10.1017/S0954579413000308; PMC3831361; PUBMED: 24229542]

Bowlby 1969

Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books, 1969.

Brazelton 1978

Brazelton TB. The Brazelton Neonatal Behavior Assessment Scale: introduction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 1978;43(5‐6):1‐13. [PUBMED: 752799]

Brazelton 1995

Brazelton TB. Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. 3rd Edition. London: Mac Keith Press, 1995.

Britt 1994a

Britt GC, Myers BJ. Testing the effectiveness of NBAS intervention with a substance‐using population. Infant Mental Health Journal 1994;15(3):293‐304. [DOI: 10.1002/1097‐0355(199423)15:3<293::AID‐IMHJ2280150305>3.0.CO;2‐E]

Britt 1994b

Britt GC, Myers BJ. The effects of Brazelton intervention: a review. Infant Mental Health Journal 1994;15(3):278‐92. [DOI: 10.1002/1097‐0355(199423)15:3<278::AID‐IMHJ2280150304>3.0.CO;2‐4]

Broussard 1970

Broussard E, Hartner MS. Maternal perception of the neonate as related to development. Child Psychiatry and Human Development 1970;1(1):16‐25. [DOI: 10.1007/BF01434585; PUBMED: 24178546]

Broussard 1971

Broussard E, Hartner M. Considerations regarding maternal perception of first born. In: Helmuth J editor(s). Exceptional Infants. Vol. 3, New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1971.

Carey 1978

Carey WB, McDevitt SC. Revision of the Infant Temperament Questionnaire. Pediatrics 1978;61:735‐9. [PUBMED: 662513]

Clarke‐Stewart 1973

Clarke‐Stewart KA. Interactions between mothers and their young children: characteristics and consequences. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 1973;38(6‐7):1‐109. [DOI: 10.2307/1165928]

Cooper 2002

Cooper PJ, Landman M, Tomlinson M, Molteno C, Swartz L, Murray L. Impact of a mother‐infant intervention in an indigent peri‐urban South African context: pilot study. British Journal of Psychiatry 2002;180:76‐81. [PUBMED: 11772856]

Cox 1987

Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10‐item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry 1987;150(6):782‐6. [DOI: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782]

Crittenden 2001

Crittenden PM. CARE‐Index: Coding Manual. Miami: Family Relations Institute, 2001.

Crockenberg 1982

Crockenberg SB, Smith P. Antecedents of mother‐infant interaction and infant irritability in the first three months of life. Infant Behavior and Development 1982;5(2‐4):105‐19. [DOI: 10.1016/S0163‐6383(82)80021‐0]

Das Eiden 1996

Das Eiden R, Reifman A. Effects of Brazelton demonstrations on later parenting: a meta‐analysis. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 1996;21(6):857‐68. [PUBMED: 8990729]

De Wolff 1997

De Wolff MS, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: a meta‐analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development 1997;68(4):571‐91. [PUBMED: 9306636]

DeGangi 2000

DeGangi GA, Breinbauer C, Doussard Roosevelt JD, Porges S, Greenspan S. Prediction of childhood problems at three years in children experiencing disorders of regulation during infancy. Journal of Infant Mental Health 2000;21(3):156‐75. [DOI: 10.1002/1097‐0355(200007)21:3<156::AID‐IMHJ2>3.0.CO;2‐D]

Dutra 2005

Dutra L, Lyons‐Ruth K. Maltreatment, maternal and child psychopathology, and quality of early care as predictors of adolescent dissociation. Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting; 2005 Apr 7‐10; Atlanta (GA). 2005.

Eshel 2006

Eshel N, Daelmans B, de Mello MC, Martines J. Responsive parenting: interventions and outcomes. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 2006;84(12):991‐8. [PMC2627571; PUBMED: 17242836]

Fearon 2010

Fearon RP, Bakermans‐Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoor MH, Lapsley AM, Roisman GI. The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children's externalizing behavior: a meta‐analytic study. Child Development 2010;81(2):435‐56. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467‐8624.2009.01405.x; PUBMED: 20438450]

Field 1978

Field T, Rydberg Dempsey J, Hayward Hallock N, Shuman HH. The mother's assessment of the behavior of her infants. Infant Behaviour and Development 1978;1:156‐67. [DOI: 10.1016/S0163‐6383(78)80026‐5]

Frankenburg 1967

Frankenburg WK, Dodds JB. The Denver Developmental Screening Test. Journal of Pediatrics 1967;71(2):181‐91. [DOI: 10.1016/S0022‐3476(67)80070‐2]

Gaertner 2007

Gaertner BM, Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Greving KA. Parental childrearing attitudes as correlates of father involvement during infancy. Journal of Marriage and Family 2007;69(4):962‐76. [DOI: 10.1111%2Fj.1741‐3737.2007.00424; NIHMS32227; PMC2174267]

GRADEpro GDT 2015 [Computer program]

McMaster University (developed by Evidence Prime). GRADEpro GDT. Version accessed 10 August 2015. Hamilton (ON): McMaster University (developed by Evidence Prime), 2015.

Granot 2001

Granot D, Mayseless O. Attachment security and adjustment to school in middle childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development 2001;25(6):530‐41. [DOI: 10.1080/01650250042000366]

Green 2002

Green J, Goldwyn R. Annotation: attachment disorganisation and psychopathology: new findings in attachment research and their potential implications for developmental psychopathology in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2002;43(7):835‐46. [PUBMED: 12405473]

Greinenberger 2005

Grienenberger JF, Kelly K, Slade A. Maternal reflective functioning, mother‐infant affective communication, and infant attachment: exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attachment and Human Development 2005;7(3):299‐311. [DOI: 10.1080/14616730500245963; PUBMED: 16210241]

Grossman 2002

Grossmann K, Grossmann K, Winter M, Zimmermann P. Attachment relationships and appraisal of partnership: from early experience of sensitive support to later relationship representation. In: Pulkkinen L, Caspi A editor(s). Paths to Successful Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2002:73‐105.

Gunnar 1996

Gunnar M, Brodersen L, Nachmias M, Buss K, Rigatuso J. Stress reactivity and attachment security. Developmental Psychobiology 1996;29(3):191‐204. [DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1098‐2302(199604)29:3<191::AID‐DEV1>3.0.CO;2‐M; PUBMED: 8666128]

Gunnar 2002

Gunnar MR, Donzella B. Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2002;27(1‐2):199‐220. [PUBMED: 11750779]

Gunnar 2007

Gunnar M, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology 2007;58:145‐73. [DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605; PUBMED: 16903808]

Hawthorne 2004

Hawthorne J. Training health professionals in the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) and its use as an intervention. The Signal: Newsletter of the World Association for Infant Mental Health2004; Vol. 12, issue 3 & 4:1‐36.

Hawthorne 2005

Hawthorne J. Using the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale to support parent‐infant relationships. Infant 2005;1(6):213‐8.

Higgins 2002

Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta‐analysis. Statistics in Medicine 2002;21(11):1539‐58. [DOI: 10.1002/sim.1186; PUBMED: 12111919]

Higgins 2016

Higgins JPT, Lasserson T, Chandler J, Tovey D, Churchill R. Methodological Expectations of Cochrane Intervention Reviews (MECIR) Version 1.05. Cochrane, 2016. Available from community.cochrane.org/mecir‐manual.

Higgins 2017

Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Sterne JAC, editor(s). Chapter 8: Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In: Higgins JPT, Churchill R, Chandler J, Cumpston MS, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.2.0 (updated June 2017). Cochrane, 2017. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.

Isabella 1994

Isabella RA. Origins of maternal role satisfaction and its influences upon maternal interactive behavior and infant‐mother attachment. Infant Behavior and Development 1994;17(4):381‐7. [DOI: 10.1016/0163‐6383(94)90030‐2]

Jacobvitz 1997

Jacobvitz D, Hazen NL, Riggs S. Disorganized mental processes in mothers, frightened/frightening behavior in caregivers, and disoriented, disorganized behavior in infancy. Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting; 1997; Washington (DC) 1997;17:329‐47.

Keren 2001

Keren M, Feldman R, Tyano S. Diagnoses and interactive patterns of infants referred to a community‐based infant mental health clinic. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2001;40(1):27‐35. [DOI: 10.1097/00004583‐200101000‐00013; PUBMED: 11195558]

Klaus 1972

Klaus MH, Jerauld R, Kreger NC, McAlpine W, Steffa M, Kennell JH. Maternal attachment: importance of the first post partum days. New England Journal of Medicine 1972;286(9):460‐3. [DOI: 10.1056/NEJM197203022860904; PUBMED: 5009748]

Koniak‐Griffin 1999

Koniak‐Griffin D, Mathenge C, Anderson NL, Verzemnieks I. An early intervention program for adolescent mothers: a nursing demonstration project. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic and Neonatal Nursing 1999;28(1):51‐9. [PUBMED: 9924864]

Koniak‐Griffin 2000

Koniak‐Griffin D, Anderson NL, Verzemnieks I, Brecht ML. A public health nursing early intervention program for adolescent mothers: outcomes from pregnancy through 6 weeks postpartum. Nursing Research 2000;49(3):130‐8. [PUBMED: 10882317]

Koniak‐Griffin 2002

Koniak‐Griffin D, Anderson NL, Brecht ML, Verzemnieks I, Lesser J, Kim S. Public health nursing care for adolescent mothers: impact on infant health and selected maternal outcomes at 1 year postbirth. Journal of Adolescent Health 2002;30(1):44‐54. [PUBMED: 11755800]

Koniak‐Griffin 2003

Koniak‐Griffin D, Verzemnieks IL, Anderson NL, Brecht ML, Lesser J, Kim S, et al. Nurse visitation for adolescent mothers: two‐year infant health and maternal outcomes. Nursing Research 2003;52(2):127‐36. [PUBMED: 12657988]

Lecce 2008

Lecce S. Attachment and Subjective Well‐Being: the Mediating Role of Emotional Processing and Regulation [PhD thesis]. Toronto: University of Toronto, 2008.

Liptak 1977

Liptak, GS, Hulka BS, Cassel JC. Effectiveness of physician‐mother interactions during infancy. Pediatrics 1977;60:186‐192. [PUBMED: 887332]

Lyons‐Ruth 2005

Lyons‐Ruth K, Yellin C, Melnick S, Atwood G. Expanding the concept of unresolved mental states: hostile/helpless states of mind on the Adult Attachment Interview are associated with disrupted mother‐infant communication and infant disorganization. Development and Psychopathology 2005;17(1):1‐23. [PMC1857275; PUBMED: 15971757]

MacDonald 2008

MacDonald HZ, Beeghly M, Grant‐Knight W, Augustyn M, Woods RW, Cabral H, et al. Longitudinal association between infant disorganized attachment and childhood posttraumatic stress symptoms. Development and Psychopathology 2008;20(2):493‐508. [DOI: 10.1017/S0954579408000242; PMC2430632; PUBMED: 18423091]

Malekpour 2007

Malekpour M. Effects of attachment on early and later development. British Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2007;53 Part 2(105):81‐95. [DOI: 10.1179/096979507799103360]

Markova 2006

Markova G, Legerstee M. Contingency, imitation, and affect sharing: foundations of infants' social awareness. Developmental Psychology 2006;42(1):132‐41. [DOI: 10.1037/0012‐1649.42.1.132; PUBMED: 16420123]

McCarthy 1972

McCarthy D. McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1972.

Meins 2001

Meins E, Fernyhough C, Fradley E, Tuckey M. Rethinking maternal sensitivity: mothers' comments on infants' mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2001;42(5):637‐48. [PUBMED: 11464968]

Meins 2012

Meins E, Fernyhough C, De Rosnay M, Arnott B, Leekam SR, Turner M. Mind‐mindedness as a multidimensional construct: appropriate and non‐attuned mind‐related comments independently predict infant‐mother attachment in a socially diverse sample. Infancy 2012;17(4):393‐415. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1532‐7078.2011.00087.x]

Moher 2009

Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta‐analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLOS Medicine 2009;6(7):e1000097. [DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097; PMC2707599 ; PUBMED: 19621072]

Myers 1982b

Myers BJ. Early intervention using Brazelton training with middle‐class mothers and fathers of newborns. Child Development 1982;53(2):462‐71. [PUBMED: 7075329]

NHS England 2014

NHS England. National Health Visiting Service Specification 2014/15. www.england.nhs.uk/wp‐content/uploads/2014/03/hv‐serv‐spec.pdf(accessed March 2014).

Nugent 2003

Nugent, JK. Home Visiting Index. Boston: Brazelton Institute Children's Hospital, 2003.

Nugent 2007

Nugent JK, Keefer CH, Minear S, Johnson LC, Blanchard Y. Understanding Newborn behaviour and Early Relationships: The Newborn Behavioral Observations (NBO) System Handbook. London: Paul H Brookes Publishing, 2007.

Nugent 2017 [pers comm]

Nugent K. Request for information on potential unpublished studies [personal communication]. Email to: J Barlow 13 June 2017.

Porges 2011

Porges SW. The Polyvagal Theory: Neurophysiological Foundations of Emotions, Attachment, Communication and Self‐regulation. New York (NY): WW Norton & Company, Inc, 2011.

Price 1977

Price GM. The Price Adaptation Scale. Unpublished manuscript1977.

Price 1983

Price, GM. Sensitivity in mother‐infant interactions: the AMIS scale. Infant Behaviour and Development 1983;6(2‐3):353‐60. [DOI: 10.1016/S0163‐6383(83)80043‐5]

Rauh 1988

Rauh VA, Achenbach TM, Nurcombe B, Howell CT, Teti DM. Minimizing adverse effects of low birthweight: four‐year results of an early intervention. Child Development 1988;59(3):544‐53. [PUBMED: 2454783]

RevMan 2014 [Computer program]

The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration. Review Manager (RevMan). Version 5.3. Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2014.

Richard 1972

Richard MPM, Bernal JF. An observational study of mother‐infant interaction. In: Blurton‐Jones N editor(s). Ethological studies of Child Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1972.

Richter 2004

Richter L. The Importance of Caregiver‐Child Interactions for the Survival and Healthy Development of Young Children: A Review. Geneva: World Health Organization, 2004.

Schore 2001

Schore AN. Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal 2001;22(1‐2):7‐66. [DOI: 10.1002/1097‐0355(200101/04)22:1<7::AID‐IMHJ2>3.0.CO;2‐N]

Schore 2002

Schore AN. Advances in neuropsychoanalysis, attachment theory, and trauma research: implications for self psychology. Psychoanalytic Inquiry 2002;22(3):433‐84. [DOI: 10.1080/07351692209348996]

Schore 2008

Schore JR, Schore AN. Modern attachment theory: the central role of affect regulation in development and treatment. Clinical Social Work Journal 2008;36(1):9‐20. [DOI: 10.1007/s10615‐007‐0111‐7]

Seashore 1973

Seashore MJ, Leifer AD, Barnett CR, Leiderman PH. The effects of denial of early mother‐infant interaction on maternal self‐confidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1973;26:369‐78.

Shea 1988

Shea E, Tronick Z. The maternal self‐report inventory: a research and clinical instrument for assessing maternal self‐esteem. In: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BE, Yogman MW editor(s). Theory and Research in Behavioral Pediatrics. New York: Plenum Press, 1988:101‐40.

Slade 2001

Slade A, Grienenberger J, Bernbach E, Levy D, Locker A. Maternal reflective functioning and attachment: considering the transmission gap. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development; 2001 April; Minneapolis (MN). 2001.

Slade 2004

Slade AAJ, Berger B, Bresgi I, Kaplan M. The Parent Development Interview ‐ Revised. New York: The City University of New York, 2004.

Slade 2005

Slade A, Grienenberger J, Bernbach E, Levy D, Locker A. Maternal reflective functioning, attachment and the transmission gap: a preliminary study. Attachment and Human Development 2005;7(3):283‐98. [DOI: 10.1080/14616730500245880; PUBMED: 16210240]

Sroufe 2005

Sroufe A, Egeland B, Carlson E, Collins W. Placing early attachment experiences in developmental context: the Minnesota Longitudinal Study. In: Grossman K, Grossmann K, Waters E editor(s). Attachment from Infancy to Adulthood. New York: The Guilford Press, 2005:48‐70.

Steele 2010

Steele H, Siever L. An attachment perspective on borderline personality disorder: advances in gene‐environment considerations. Current Psychiatry Reports 2010;12(1):61‐7. [DOI: 10.1007/s11920‐009‐0091‐0; PUBMED: 20425312]

Stern 1998

Stern DN, Bruschweiler‐Stern N, Freeland A. The Birth of a Mother: How the Motherhood Experience Changes You Forever. New York (NY): Basic Books, 1998.

Sumner 1994

Sumner G, Spietz A. NCAST Caregiver/Parent‐Child Interaction Feeding Manual. Seattle: University of Washington, 1994.

Teti 1996

Teti DM, O'Connell MA, Reiner CD. Parenting sensitivity, parental depression and child health: the mediational role of parental self‐efficacy. Infant and Child Development 1996;5(4):237‐50. [DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1099‐0917(199612)5:4<237::AID‐EDP136>3.0.CO;2‐5]

Vondra 2001

Vondra JI, Shaw DS, Swearingen L, Cohen M, Owens EB. Attachment stability and emotional and behavioral regulation from infancy to preschool age. Development and Psychopathology 2001;13(1):13‐33. [PUBMED: 11346048]

Waters 2000

Waters E, Merrick S, Treboux D, Crowell J, Albersheim L. Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: a twenty‐year longitudinal study. Child Development 2000;71(3):684‐9. [PUBMED: 10953934]

WAVE Trust 2013

WAVE Trust, Department of Education. Conception to age 2 ‐ the age of opportunity: framework for local area service commissioners. www.wavetrust.org/sites/default/files/reports/conception‐to‐age‐2‐framework_0.pdf(accessed March 2013).

Weinfield 2000

Weinfield NS, Sroufe LA, Egeland B. Attachment from infancy to early adulthood in a high‐risk sample: continuity, discontinuity, and their correlates. Child Development 2000;71(3):695‐702. [PUBMED: 10953936]

Worobey 1985

Worobey J. A review of Brazelton‐based interventions to enhance parent‐infant interaction. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology 1985;3(2):64‐73. [DOI: 10.1080/02646838508403464]

Bartram 2016

Bartram SC, Barlow J, Wolke D. The Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) and Newborn Behavioral Observations system (NBO) for supporting caregivers and improving outcomes in caregivers and their infants. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2015, Issue 6. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD011754]

Characteristics of studies

Characteristics of included studies [ordered by study ID]

Anderson 1981

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 30

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of the 3 treatment groups: control (group I), explanation only (group II), demonstration only (group III)

Number randomised to each group: 10

Participant mothers:

  1. All mothers had uncomplicated pregnancies and normal labours and deliveries. The Obstetrical Complications Scale was used in selecting participant mothers. To be included in the study, mothers had to achieve a score of 85% or greater (mean 96%).

  2. Married, white and classified as middle class according to the Hollingshead Two‐Factor Index

  3. Had local or regional anaesthesia for delivery; no mother received more than 75 mg of analgesia

  4. Breastfed their infants

  5. Mean age 25.23 years (range 19 to 36 years)

  6. Mean education 15.17 years (range 12 to 20 years)

  7. Maternal occupation (according to classification of occupations using the Hollingshead Two‐Factor Index: mean 3.2, mode 3, range 1 to 7

Participant infants:

  1. First‐born girls

  2. Developed appropriately for gestational age

  3. Placed in full‐term nursery following birth

  4. Mean weight 3402 g (range 2567 to 4992)

  5. Assessed for normalcy with the Postnatal Complications Scale and received a score of 10 (indicating no complications)

Interventions

Control group: while the NBAS was performed on infants of mothers in this group, the mothers received no immediate feedback on their infants. Instead they received a class on infant furnishings.

Explanation only: NBAS was performed on the infants, and following each assessment the investigator met with mother and explained the performance of the infant.

Demonstration group: mothers observed the investigator performing NBAS on their infants and saw both stimuli and responses.The explanations were ongoing rather than at the end of session.

Outcomes

Study outcomes: Price Adaptation Scale (PAS). A 21‐item instrument designed to measure the quality of reciprocity in mother‐infant interaction.

Timing of outcome assessment: 10 days postpartum

Outcome observer: a graduate student had been trained with the PAS rating system; this observer was unaware of treatment group to which the mothers were assigned. First assessment was 24‐48 h following delivery (pre‐test) and second observation was made at 10 days postpartum. Assessment was done between groups I vs III, groups I vs II, and group II vs III to find out whether instructions received about their neonate's capacities and behaviours influence the caregiver‐infant interaction at 10 to 12 days postpartum.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Eligible mother‐infant pairs were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 treatment groups; however, the method of sequence generation was not described.

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Method of allocation concealment was not described.

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information was provided about whether the mothers in the study knew about their own or other treatment conditions.

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

The raters were unaware of the treatment group to which the mothers were assigned.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

No missing data

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes reported

Other bias

Low risk

None

Beal 1989

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 44

Fathers were assigned to 1 of the 2 groups: experimental, control

Number randomised to each group: 22

Participant fathers:

  1. First‐time fathers

  2. Attended prenatal classes

  3. Present during labor and delivery

  4. Mostly white, working‐class men

  5. Mean age: 28 years

  6. Mean years of education: 14 years

Analysis of variance revealed no significant socioeconomic differences between the control and experimental groups.

Participant infants: not specified

Interventions

Experimental group: the researcher demonstrated the NBAS to father and mother together. Immediate and continuing feedback as well as a terminal summary statement was given to the parents during the examination.

Control group: NBAS was performed on the infants by the researcher, and a summary statement was given to the parents. No actual demonstration was provided.

Outcomes

Study instruments:

  1. Paternal Attitude Scale: consists of 38 Likert items on a 5‐point bipolar scale. Used to measure paternal attitude toward infant caretaking

  2. Infant Characteristics Questionnaire: consists of 28 Likert items. Used to measure father's perception of the degree of difficult temperament of his infant

Timing of outcome assessment: 8 weeks postpartum (as a home visit).

Outcome observer: first demonstration of NBAS was carried out 2 to 3 days following delivery (pre‐test), and second observation was made at 8 weeks postpartum. Assessment was undertaken to identify whether demonstrating NBAS would affect quality and frequency of father‐infant interaction, paternal attitude towards caretaking, paternal involvement in caretaking, perception of infant difficultness and quality of interaction.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

High risk

Eligible fathers were randomly assigned to experimental or control group using alternating allocation

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

High risk

Blinding of outcome assessors was not undertaken. NBAS demonstrations and postpartum home visits were both undertaken by the principal investigator.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Beeghly 1995a

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 125

Mothers were assigned to 1 of 2 groups: experimental (infant‐centred, NBAS‐based), control (mother‐centred; interview‐based)

Number randomised to each group: 59 experimental, 66 control

Participant mothers:

  1. English‐speaking

  2. Had residences equipped with a telephone

  3. Lived within an hour's drive from the laboratory

  4. Had no plans to move within the next 2 years

  5. Had infants who met the inclusion criteria for the infants

  6. Average maternal age: 28.6 years

  7. 62% primiparous

  8. 71% white

  9. 79% married

Participant infants:

  1. Full‐term delivery (38‐42 weeks gestational age)

  2. Infants age between 2 and 4 days postbirth; "clinically normal" with no major congenital abnormalities as determined by routine paediatric examination on day 1

  3. No more than 24 hours in the neonatal special care nursery

  4. Infants were selected according to their physical size at the time of delivery. Half of the infants were intrauterine growth retarded, as defined by small for gestational age (detected when the Ponderal Index at the time of delivery fell below the 10th percentile for gestational age) and half were average for gestational age (Ponderal Index fell between 25 and 90 percentiles for gestational age)

  5. 50% boys

Interventions

Experimental group: at day 3 in the hospital, and again at days 14 and 30 at home, the mother was invited to become a "participant observer" as the infant was examined with the NBAS by an experienced clinician with established reliability in using the NBAS as preventive intervention in the newborn period. After the administration of the NBAS, the examiner provided a narrative summary of the assessment, encouraging the parent to ask or discuss any questions, concerns or related issues.

Control group: an experienced clinician interviewed the mother at days 3, 14 and 30, while a second, trained, reliable examiner administered and scored the NBAS with the infant in a separate room. After the interview, the mother was given a general summary of her infant's performance on the NBAS by the examiner, excluding specific guidelines for caregiving based on the observations of the infant during the NBAS.

Outcomes

Study instruments:

  1. Parenting Stress Index, which yields 2 summary scores: total parenting stress from child‐related sources and total parenting stress from parental sources

  2. Mother‐infant interactive behaviour, during face‐to‐face play with toys, as coded with video tapes

  3. Bayley Scales of Infant Development, which yields 2 standard scores: mental Development Index and Psychomotor Development Index

Timing of outcome assessment: at 4 months age of the infant (at a laboratory).

Outcome observer: Intervention protocols were administered on 3 successive days during the first month postpartum; at 3 days in the hospital and at 14 and 30 days at home. 3 touch points during the first month were implemented to allow 3 successive NBAS administration. Final outcome assessed at 4 months in the laboratory.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: grant R01 MH37234 from the National Institute of Mental Health, grant from the William Randolph Hearst Foundation and grant from the Mailman Family Foundation to TB Brazelton (PI)

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Eligible mothers were randomly assigned to 1 of the 2 intervention groups; method of sequence generation not described

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

Blinding was undertaken; coders were blind to infant IUGR and maternal intervention status.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Belsky 1985

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 67

Participants assigned to 1 of 4 groups: mother and father (experimental group I); only mother (experimental group II), mother and father (control group I); only mother (control group II)

Number randomised to each group: 15 in each experimental group; 15 in each control group (after attrition)

Participant both parents or mothers only:

  1. All couples were from intact marriages

  2. All couples were bearing their first child

  3. Middle class

  4. Annual family income found in the last trimester of pregnancy averaged USD 22,475

  5. Couples had been married at the time of the interview for an average of 4.05 years

  6. For 2 of every 3 couples, the first birth was planned

  7. Mean age: 26.6 years mothers; 28.1 years fathers

  8. Education of 2 parents averaged 14.6 and 15.7 years (mentioned as given in article; no further elaboration)

Participant infants: not described

Interventions

On 2nd, 3rd or 4th day of the infant's life and following the assessment of each newborn on the NBAS by a certified examiner, families participating in the project were allocated to 1 of 4 treatment groups.

Experimental groups: actively elicited from their newborns a series of reflexes and behaviours culled from the NBAS under the guidance of the experimenter, now serving as facilitator – the same individual who had previously administered the NBAS to the infant. The facilitator neither touched nor held the infant during these sessions; rather she guided the parent through the procedure, describing what the parents were to do, alerting them as to what to look for, asking them what they observed, and discussing the significance of the behavioural phenomena under examination.

Control group: received a detailed verbal report of the exam and their babies' performance from the examiner/facilitator. More specifically, the examiner described in step‐by‐step fashion exactly what she did during the exam, how the baby behaved, and the meaning of the infant's responses.

Outcomes

Study instruments used

  1. Joint couple interview and individual spousal questionnaire; during prenatal period and 3 and 9 months postpartum

  2. Newborn behavioural assessment at local hospital and file review of newborn's medical records; carried out within a few days of infant's birth

  3. Naturalistic home observations for 2 h at 1, 3 and 9 months postpartum. Caregiver‐infant interactions (mother‐infant dyad) were observed using a behaviour checklist. Caregiver perception of infant (mother‐father‐infant triad) observed using a detailed behavioural checklist

Timing of outcome assessment: during the prenatal phase of the project (preintervention) and again at 1, 3 and 9 months postpartum

Outcome observer: Intervention protocols were administered on second, third or fourth day and outcomes observed at 1, 3 and 9 months postpartum to find out whether NBAS performed under the guidance of the experimenter yielded better results than control condition.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Eligible families were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 intervention groups; method of sequence generation was not described.

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Low risk

The individual scheduling the prenatal visit was unaware of the group to which each family was assigned.

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

Blinding was achieved by providing the scheduler with a packet of manila folders in a specified order. Each time a prenatal visit was scheduled, the top folder in the stack was selected and the family's name and address were placed on the folder. The folders had been pre‐coded for treatment condition.

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

The only people with knowledge of group assignment were: the author, who did not collect any of the data in the investigation, and the prenatal visitor, who also administered the Brazelton exam and the intervention.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

High risk

Attrition: 7 out of 67 participants. Participant attrition was not selective, either as a function of group assignment or background factors. No intention‐to‐treat analysis

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Britt 1994

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Tota number randomised: 54

Participants assigned to 1 of 4 groups: experimental group I (used only NBAS), experimental group II (MABI), experimental group III (NBAS plus MABI), control group (NBAS not used on babies). Experimental groups II and III were not taken for calculation purposes.

Number randomised to each group: 13 experimental, 13 control

Participant mothers: all drug‐using mothers

Characteristics of the participant both parents/mothers only:

  1. All drug‐using mothers. Drug use for 46 (85.2%) participants verified through a positive urine toxicology on either infant or the mother. Most of the women (92.6%) used cocaine, either alone or with another drug

  2. Mean age of mothers: 26 years (SD 3.8)

Participant infants: no information

Interventions

Experimental group: Brazelton only; mothers were taught how to elicit items from the Brazelton exam on their own infants.

Control group: the experimenter talked with the mother about the conditions of the study and about her baby.

Outcomes

Study instruments used:

  1. Nursing Child Assessment Feeding Scale, used to measure mother‐infant interaction during a home visit at 4 weeks

  2. 3‐item Likert‐type scale used to measure maternal receptivity

  3. Series of questions derived from NBAS used to measure the maternal knowledge of infant behaviour

Timing of outcome assessment: at 4 weeks (during a home visit)

Outcome observer: a blind observer scored a feeding session and rated various aspects using the study instruments

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: Virginia Commonwealth University (VCU) Faculty Grant‐in‐Aid awarded to the second author, and National Insitute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) Grant DA‐06094

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Eligible families were randomly assigned to 1 of the 4 intervention groups; no information provided about sequence generation

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

A blinded observer scored feeding sessions and carried out the rating.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Furr 1982

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 40

Participants assigned to 1 of 4 groups: maternal orientation to newborn behaviour using Infant Behaviour Assessment Record (IBAR) adapted from NBAS (intervention‐ group III); feeding observation and maternal orientation to newborn behaviour using IBAR adapted from NBAS (group I); only feeding observation (group II); no action (group IV)

Number randomised to each group: 10 in each group

Participant mothers:

  1. Primiparous mothers

  2. Absence of significant perinatal risk factors

  3. Only breastfeeding mothers included

  4. Mothers between 18 and 30 years of age

Participant infants:

  1. Multiple births, congenital anomalies such as Down's, neural tube defects, Pierre‐Robin excluded

  2. Gestational age between 38‐42 weeks

  3. APGAR score 7‐10 at both 1 and 5 minutes

  4. Appropriate for gestational age babies

Interventions

Experimental (group III): used IBAR adapted from NBAS, as a guideline for orienting the mothers to their newborn behaviour on third day.

Group I: instructed to feed baby as if no one were present at day 2 and on day 3 maternal orientation as for group III.

Group II: only feeding observation on day 2

Group IV: no action

Outcomes

Study instruments used:

  1. Nursing Child Assessment Feeding Scale (NCAFS), used to measure mother‐infant interaction

Timing of outcome assessment: at 2 weeks (during a home visit)

Outcome observer: each mother was asked to breastfeed her baby. Scoring of NCAFS was completed following each observation

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Low risk

Participant mothers were assigned a group number from a cyclic rotating table of numbers.

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

High risk

Investigator singularly involved in all phases of data collection ‐ random allocation, pre‐testing, behavioural orientation and post‐testing

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Killough 2004

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 40

Participants were assigned to 1 of the 2 groups: experimental (used NBAS on babies), control (NBAS not used on babies)

Number randomised to each group: 20 experimental, 20 control

Participant mothers:

  1. Primiparous mothers

  2. Had no perinatal complications

  3. No history of depression

  4. Mean age 29 (SD 6.8) years experimental group; 28 (SD 6.5) years control group

  5. All mothers had completed high school or high school equivalency

Participant infants:

  1. Healthy, full‐term, newborn infants

  2. Gestational age between 38 and 41 weeks

  3. APGAR scores of no less than 6 at 1 minute, 8 at 5 minutes and 9 at 10 minutes

  4. Sex: 12 females (63%) experimental group; 8 females (47%) control group

Interventions

Experimental group: NBAS session was conducted on babies in the presence of the mother

Control group: NBAS was not conducted

Outcomes

Study instruments used:

  1. Maternal Representations Questionnaire, used by mothers to rate the perceptions of their babies and of themselves as mother within 2 days of birth; at 2, 3 and 4 weeks after birth; and finally at 4 months after birth

  2. CARE‐Index (Crittenden 2001), designed to assess level of sensitivity in parent infant interaction (4 months after delivery)

Timing of outcome assessment: within 2 days of delivery, at 3 to 4 weeks after delivery and finally at 4 months after delivery

Outcome observer: a researcher blind to the status of the family in the research assessed the final outcome at 4 months after delivery.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Eligible mother‐infant dyads were randomly assigned to 1 of the 4 intervention groups; sequence generation was not described

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

A blinded researcher assessed the final session and carried out the rating.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

High risk

3 control dyads and 1 experimental dyad completed participation through time 2 but not time 3. Therefore, follow‐up sample size comprised of 17 participant dyads in the control group and 19 participant dyads in the experimental group. No intention‐to‐treat analysis

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Liptak 1983

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 75

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of 3 treatment groups: experimental, control group I, control group II

Number randomised to each group: 25

Participant mothers:

  1. English speaking

  2. Middle‐class families

Participant infants:

  1. Healthy newborn infants

  2. Full term

  3. First‐born

  4. White

Interventions

Experimental group: parents received NBAS; parts of this examination were demonstrated to the mothers on the morning of their discharge, approximately 30 minutes following a feeding. A semi‐structured discussion modified for the individuality of each child was held with mothers and included an assessment of the child's strengths, individual differences and the importance of mutual interaction.

Control group I: infants received standard newborn care. Physical examinations were performed in the absence of parents. Mothers were visited by one of the authors every day during stay, and any concerns raised by the mothers were addressed.

Control group II: infants received standard care + complete physical examinations in front of the mothers on the morning of discharge. During the examination the physician discussed the normal physical findings and emphasised the robustness of the infants.

Outcomes

Study instruments used:

  1. Neonatal Perception Inventory, to assess mother's (caregiver) perception of her infant with that of the average baby with respect to crying, feeding, vomiting, sleeping, bowel movements and predictability of eating and sleeping patterns

  2. Ainsworth Interaction Scale. Observers rated mother‐child interaction using this 9‐point scale, which assesses the general attitude of the mother toward her child and her own role, and examines areas such as feeding, availability for interaction, physical contact, response to crying and social contact

  3. Modified Degree of Bother Scale, used to determine the extent the child's activities such as crying, sleeping, feeding, lack of regular pattern and stubbornness bothered the mother

  4. Carey Infant Temperament Questionnaire, which consists of 70 items descriptive of infant behaviour in a variety of settings and is scored to obtain ratings on 9 temperamental characteristics

Timing of outcome assessment: at 1 and 3 months of age; for 35 minutes at 1 month and for 45 minutes at 3 months

Outcome observer: an observer blind to the group status assessed the outcome.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: supported by a biomedical research support grant from the University of Rochester and by a grant from the Commonwealth Fund (No. 74‐26)

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Eligible newborns were randomly assigned to 1 of the 3 groups; method of sequence generation was not described

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

An observer blind to the group status assessed the outcome.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

Out of the 75 who agreed to participate, 3 families dropped out of the study (2 before the first home visit, and 1 after the first home visit). No intention‐to‐treat analysis

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

McManus 2012

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: recruitment of infants January‐August 2004

Participants

Total number randomised: 41

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of 2 treatment groups: intervention, control

Number randomised to each group: 25 intervention, 13 control (after attrition)

Participant mothers: fluent in English

Participant infants:

  1. All infants at risk for developmental delay

  2. Referred to 1 of the 3 early intervention programmes in 1 state during the study period

  3. Less than 12 weeks corrected age

  4. Parent was the infant's primary caregiver

Interventions

Experimental group: received 7 home visits from an early‐intervention provider certified in the NBO system, who administered the NBO with the parents and discussed characteristics of the infant. The final visit took place no later than the end of 12th week of the infant's life.

Control group: received traditional, early intervention, home‐based delivery; 7 home visits from an early‐intervention provider not certified in the NBO

Outcomes

Study instruments used: Home Visiting Index (HVI). A 25‐item scale, which requests the parents to rate, on a 4‐point scale, their degree of agreement with statements about the quality of early intervention service delivery

Timing of outcome assessment: at the end of the third month

Outcome observer: the same early intervention service provider who gave the HVI to the parents at the end of the third month

Notes

Conflicts of interests: both authors disclose their affiliation with the Brazelton Institute, Department of Newborn Medicine, Children's Hospital Boston. The second author is Director of the Brazelton Institute and assisted in the design of the neurobehavioural intervention under study.

Source of funding: Robert Wood Johnson Health & Society Scholars Program at University of Wisconsin‐Madison and the Noonan Family Foundation

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Participants were randomly assigned to either NBO or usual care group using "simple randomisation procedures" – sequence generation was not provided.

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

High risk

Of the 41 infants recruited, 3 were not followed due to inability to contact the family after initial referral. No intention‐to‐treat analysis

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

High risk

Both authors disclose their affiliation with the Brazelton Institute, Department of Newborn Medicine, Children's Hospital Boston. The second author is Director of the Brazelton Institute and assisted in the design of the neurobehavioural intervention under study.

Myers 1982a

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 42

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of the 3 treatment groups: father treatment (group 1), mother treatment (group II), control (group III)

Number randomised to each group: father treatment, 14; mother treatment, 14; control group, 12 (after attrition)

Participant parents:

  1. Had their first baby

  2. Had a minimum of a high school education (mean 15.5 years father, 14.4 years mother)

  3. At least 20 years old (mean age 30.8 years father, 28.0 years mother)

Participant infants:

  1. All infants were full‐term

  2. Mean weight 4056 g, all infants over 2267 g (5 lb)

  3. Healthy, vaginal deliveries without complications

  4. APGAR ratings were at least 8 at 1 and 5 minutes

  5. Sex: 20 males and 22 females

Interventions

Experimental groups: a training session based on the NBAS with either the mother (mother treatment) or the father (father treatment) actively learning to administer the items and to observe her or his own infant's behaviour. Parents were told that these were some "games and exercises" to get to know their baby better. The treatment was given no sooner than the second day. The sessions were held in the mother's room for mothers and in a small room off the nursery for fathers.

Control group: parents did not receive the training session or know that it existed.

Outcomes

Study instruments used:

  1. 15 multiple‐choice factual questions. These questions were related to the infant's physical capacities, including reflexes and senses, and were used to measure the caregiver knowledge of infant behaviour

  2. Broussard Neonatal Perception Inventory. This test compared the perception of the average baby of 1 month with the baby in the study on a 5‐point scale (caregiver perception of infant)

Timing of outcome assessment: in the hospital, at least 6 hours after the treatment, and at home at 4 weeks

Outcome observer: the hospital measures were collected by 3 female assistants who were blind to group membership. The 4‐week measures consisted of questionnaires that were mailed to the participant's homes. Except for the assessment of father's caretaking, all measures were performed on both parents.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Low risk

All parents who met the criteria were randomly assigned to a treatment group by the throw of a die and did not differ in terms of parents' age and education or infant birth weight.

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

Parents unaware of other treatment groups

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

Measures were collected by assistants who were blind to group membership. 4‐week measure included a mailed questionnaire

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

2 members in the control group were lost to follow‐up at assessment at 4 weeks postpartum. No intention‐to‐treat analysis

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Nugent 2014

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: participants recruited between December and January 2001

Participants

Total number randomised: 112

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of the 2 groups: intervention, control

Number randomised to each group: 57 (final stage 55) intervention, 55 (final stage 51) control

Participant mothers: first‐time parent cohabiting with the father of the baby

Participant infants:

  1. Vaginal deliveries between 36 and 42 weeks' gestational age

  2. APGAR scores not less than 7 (at 5 minutes and 15 minutes)

  3. No congenital anomalies

  4. No use of neonatal intensive care unit

Interventions

Intervention group: mothers received routine care plus NBO in the hospital at mothers' bedside and again at 1 month, postpartum home visit

Control group: mothers received routine hospital care and a short‐attention, control home visit

Outcomes

Study instruments used: Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) – a 10‐item, self‐report questionnaire to assess symptoms of postnatal depression during the previous week

Timing of outcome assessment: at 1 month postpartum

Outcome observer: EPDS was a self‐administered questionnaire at 1 month postpartum; not mentioned whether it was administered by an investigator blind to the group status or not

Notes

No conflict of interest: author is director of the Brazelton Institute

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Low risk

A coin toss was used to randomly allocate eligible mothers to intervention and control groups.

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

High risk

112 mothers had enrolled for the study, but at 1 month postpartum, there were only 106 mothers. No intention‐to‐treat analysis

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

High risk

Author is director of the Brazelton Institute

Nugent 2015

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 40

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of the 2 groups: intervention, control

Number randomised to each group: 20 (final stage 18) intervention, 20 (final stage 17) control

Participant mothers: first‐time parent living with the father of the baby

Participant infants:

  1. Vaginal deliveries between 36 and 42 weeks' gestational age

  2. Birth weights above 2500 grams

  3. APGAR scores 7 or greater (by 10 minutes)

  4. No congenital anomalies

  5. No use of neonatal intensive care unit

  6. Not included if the infants were circumcised that day

  7. Not included if lived far away from hospital

Interventions

Intervention group: mothers received routine hospital care plus NBO in the hospital and again at 1 month, postpartum home visit plus NBO (for second time). Home visit at 4 months postpartum.

Control group: mothers received routine hospital care and home visit at 1 month and 4 months.

Outcomes

Study instruments used: the CARE‐Index

Timing of outcome assessment: at 4 months postpartum

Outcome observer: CARE‐Index was administered in the home by an examiner who had been trained in its administration and coding and who was blind to the status of the dyad.

Notes

Conclict of interest: author is director of the Brazelton Institute

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Mothers who consented to participate were randomly assigned either to intervention or control group. Method of sequence generation was not described.

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

Final outcome was assessed by an examiner who was blind to the status of the dyad

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

High risk

40 mothers were initially allocated to the study, but at 4 months postpartum only 35 mother‐infant dyads remained.

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

High risk

Author is director of the Brazelton Institute

Pannabecker 1982

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 48

Father‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of the 3 groups: experimental (group I), hospital control (group II), office control (group III)

Number randomised to each group: 16

Participant fathers: married, older, white and college educated; they were skilled or professionally employed. These fathers were well prepared for childbirth, since most couples had attended the Lamaze childbirth education classes, and all couples had attended some form of prenatal classes.

Participant infants:

  1. Met criteria for normality, which included an uncomplicated pregnancy and delivery

  2. Birth weight between 2500 grams and 4100 grams

  3. 1 minute and 5 minute APGAR rating of 7 or more

  4. Normal physical examination

Interventions

Experimental group: 2 × 30‐minute sessions were held with each father during the postpartum hospitalisation. The first when the infant was 12‐36 hours old and the second approximately 24 hours after the first session. These sessions were expressly for fathers, and mothers were not included. At the first session, a number of physical characteristics of the infant were pointed out. Some exercises were demonstrated and the father was given time to hold and interact with his infant. At the second session, the father was given an opportunity to hold and interact with his infant after demonstration of a standard number of behavioural items from NBAS.

Hospital control group: these fathers spent the same amount of time with the investigator at the same postnatal time points and discussed the same information, but with a major difference. All information was presented through a prepared videotape of a typical normal newborn.

Office control group: these fathers had no prior contact with the experiment except for the recruiting visit in the hospital. No educational information and no extra contact with their infants.

Outcomes

Study instruments used: direct observations of behaviours made during infant's physical examination and subsequent ratings of behaviours made from videotape segments – these were sensitive measures of father‐infant interaction.

Timing of outcome assessment: at 4 weeks of age of the infant.

Outcome observer: infant was examined by the paediatric nurse practitioner while the baby was with the father. Selected father and infant behaviours were observed during the physical examination by an observer blind to the group status.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: supported in part by a Nurse‐Scientist pre‐doctoral fellowship (US Public Health Service (USPHS), Division of Nursing, Grant NU‐5008‐05) and a grant from the Developmental Psychobiology Endowment Fund, Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Medical Center. Dr Emde is supported by Research Scientist Award.

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

First 2 groups (I and II) were randomly assigned to an experimental group or a hospital control group; method of sequence generation was not described.

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

Outcome assessed by an observer unaware of the groups to which the participants belonged

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Poley‐Strobel 1987

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 20

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of 2 groups: experimental, control

Number randomised to each group: 10

Participant mothers:

  1. Black

  2. Primipara

  3. Vaginal delivery

  4. Low socioeconomic status

  5. Uneventful pregnancy, labour, delivery and postpartum course

  6. 18 of the mothers were single and 2 were married (both married mothers were in the control group)

Participant infants:

  1. Full‐term

  2. Singleton birth

  3. Bottle‐fed

  4. No major congenital anomalies

  5. No major perinatal complications

Interventions

Experimental group: the intervention involved a teaching modelling session in which the investigator explained and demonstrated the infant's behavioural cues and responses to the mother using NBAS. The teaching plan was structured around 4 areas: interactive processes, motoric processes, organisation processes‐state control, and organisation processes‐response to stress. All of the interventions were scheduled around 1 of the infant's feeding times so that the infant would be awake and could be used in modelling the behaviours being discussed.

Control group: NBAS was not used

Outcomes

Study instruments used: assessment of Mother‐Infant Sensitivity. Mother‐infant interaction was measured using this instrument.

Timing of outcome assessment: 10 to 14 days following delivery

Outcome observer: study intervention was carried out 1 to 3 days after discharge from hospital in the first instance. An appointment was made with all mothers for a home visit 10‐14 days following delivery to observe another feeding session with the infant and obtain the post‐test data. The NBAS was also administered.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Unclear risk

Randomly assigned to the control or the experimental group; no information provided about sequence generation

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

An observer unaware of the assignment of the mother to a group observed the feeding for the mother‐infant interaction.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported

Other bias

Low risk

None

Szajnberg 1987

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Number randomised: 25

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of 2 groups: intervention, control

Number randomised to each group: 12 intervention, 13 control

Participant mothers: not described

Participant infants:

  1. 28 to 32 weeks

  2. Appropriate for gestational age (AGA) infants when they reached 34 weeks gestational age

  3. No congenital anomalies or bilateral intraventricular or parenchymal haemorrhages before 34 weeks gestational age

Interventions

Intervention group: at 34 weeks of age, intervention group mothers were invited to observe NBAS performed on their infants by a trained examiner with a second observer/recorder. Mothers were invited to ask questions during and after the examination.

Control group: mothers were invited to observe a standard complete physical performed by an attending neonatologist and a second observer/recorder. Mothers were invited to ask questions during and after the examination.

Outcomes

Study instruments:

  1. Cohler Maternal Attitude Scale: a Likert‐type, 233‐item scale, which measures mothers' attitudes towards child rearing at various developmental ages up to 36 months and along several issues. This was initially assessed before the intervention and then at 6 months corrected age.

  2. Carey Infant Temperament Questionnaire: a parent‐report measure of infant temperament (caregiver perception of infant) at 6 months corrected age.

  3. Bayley Scales of Infant Development: to assess infant development (at 6 months corrected age)

Timing of outcome assessment: at 6 months corrected age of the baby

Outcome observer: at 6 months corrected age. Mothers were interviewed using a semi‐structured format.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Low risk

Mother‐infant pairs were randomly assigned to control and intervention groups; no information provided about sequence generation

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

No information provided

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

High risk

One control group participant refused follow‐up, and 4 were lost to follow‐up. They did not differ in income, demographic or medical characteristics. No intention‐to‐treat analysis

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

Worobey 1982

Methods

Design: parallel‐group, randomised trial

Unit of allocation: participant

Study dates: not reported

Participants

Total number randomised: 48

Mother‐infant pairs assigned to 1 of 3 groups: experimental treatment (group I), control group (group II), or contrast treatment (group III)

Number randomised to each group: 16

Participant mothers: white, middle‐class

Participant infants: healthy, full‐term newborns

Interventions

All infants were assessed using NBAS initially.

Experiment treatment: active information through auditory, visual and tactile interaction. Mothers individually observed a 45‐minute demonstration of the NBAS conducted on their newborns, on second or third day of life after the examiner had assessed the newborn on NBAS before discharge from the hospital. The examiner, serving as facilitator, neither touched nor held the infant during these sessions; rather he talked the mother through the procedures and guided and encouraged her in eliciting infant responses.

Control treatment: minimal information through auditory feedback. Immediately after the behavioural assessment of the newborn, the mother received a verbal summary of her infant's performance. The examiner described the baby's performance on the 4 categories of the profile, according to the priori cluster, and discussed the baby's performance with the new mother. This procedure allowed the investigator to alert the mother to characteristics of her newborn, based on its behavioural display, and provided the mother with the opportunity to talk about what she heard.

Contrast treatment: passive observation with auditory and visual feedback. Mothers observed a 45‐minute demonstration of the NBAS conducted on their newborns. This procedure allowed the investigator to alert the mother to the characteristics of her newborn, based on its behavioural display, and also drew attention to specific infant behaviours and provided the mother with an opportunity to talk about what she was seeing. These mothers received a 1‐page summary of the demonstration 2 days later.

Outcomes

Study instruments: pre‐coded checklist to assess mother‐infant interaction.

Timing of outcome assessment: 4 and 6 weeks of age for the infant.

Outcome observer: mother‐infant dyads were observed for 60 minutes on 1 occasion in the family's home, by 1 of the observers blind not only to experimental design but also to the purpose of the study.

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: grant from the Penn State Division of Continuing Education

Risk of bias

Bias

Authors' judgement

Support for judgement

Random sequence generation (selection bias)

Low risk

Randomly assigned to 1 of the 3 treatment conditions; no information provided about sequence generation

Allocation concealment (selection bias)

Unclear risk

No information provided

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)
All outcomes

High risk

The experimenter explicitly avoided telling mothers that the service provided would affect their parenting or their baby. However, one investigator administered all 3 treatments and thus was cognisant of group assignments.

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)
All outcomes

Low risk

Observers were blind not only to the experimental design but also to the purpose of the study.

Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias)
All outcomes

Unclear risk

No information provided

Selective reporting (reporting bias)

Low risk

All pre‐specified outcomes were reported.

Other bias

Low risk

None

APGAR: appearance, pulse, grimace, activity and respiration;IUGR: intrauterine growth retardation;MABI: Mother's Assessment of the Behaviour of her Infant; NBAS: Neonatal Behavioural Assessment Scale; NBO: Newborn Behavioural Observations system; PI: principal investigator; SD: standard deviation.

Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by study ID]

Study

Reason for exclusion

Cardone 1990

A derived "specialized application of NBAS"

Cooper 2009

NBAS used as part of a wider intervention

Cooper 2015

Used a variation of NBAS

Egeland 1984

Not a randomised controlled trial

Fowles 1999

Not a randomised controlled trial

Golas 1986

NBAS used as part of intervention with the Amazing Newborn DVD

Gomes 1987

Intervention 'based on' NBAS

Gomespedro 1995

NBAS part of broader package of care

Hart 1998

NBAS used in conjunction with MABI.

Joshi 2013

Non‐randomised controlled trial; both control and intervention groups given NBAS

Kiepura 2011

Non‐randomised controlled trial

Kusaka 2007

Non‐randomised controlled trial

Meisels 1993

Qualitative report of outcomes only (i.e. no date)

Nurcombe 1984

NBAS used as part of intervention package – MITP.

Ogi 2001

Intervention 'based on' NBAS; non‐randomised

Ohgi 2004

NBAS used as part of wider package of care

Parker 1992

Uses Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Programme (NIDCAP) and Assessment of Preterm Infants' Behaviour (APIB) rather than NBAS

Parker Loewen 1987

Does not appear to be the NBAS

Sanders 2006

Not a randomised controlled trial

Sullivan 1980

No information about how NBAS was used.

Teti 2009

NBAS used as part of wider intervention

Wendland Carro 1999

NBAS demonstrated on video

Widmayer 1980

NBAS as part of package with the MABI.

Widmayer 1981

NBAS as part of wider intervention package.

MABI: Mother's Assessment of the Behaviour of her Infant; MITP: Mother Infant Transaction Program; NBAS: Neonatal Behavioural Assessment Scale.

Characteristics of ongoing studies [ordered by study ID]

NCT03070652

Trial name or title

What are the effects of supporting early parenting by increasing the understanding of the infant? A randomized community‐based trial

Methods

Design: cluster‐randomised trial

Participants

Study population: new families.

  1. First‐time parents: around 40%

  2. Given premature birth: around 6%

  3. Twin birth: 2%

  4. Birth depression in the postnatal period in mothers: 10% to 14%

  5. Birth depression in the postnatal period in fathers: 7% to 8%

  6. Cultural background other than Danish depending on the geographical area: 7% to 20%

Exclusion criteria: parents or infants affiliated to special treatment elsewhere

Interventions

Intervention group: will receive NBO at 3 weeks after birth and in following visits up to 3 months

Control group: will receive care as usual at 3 weeks after birth and in following visits up to 3 months

Outcomes

  1. Self‐reported data from new parents collected via questionnaire

  2. Administrative data from health visitor's daily monitoring systems

  3. Parent‐infant interaction as measured by CARE‐Index

Starting date

1 January 2017

Contact information

Hanne Kronborg, Aarhus University, Denmark. E‐mail: [email protected]

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Trials register number:NCT03070652

Nicolson 2016

Trial name or title

Understanding your newborn and adapting to parenthood (UNA): a randomised clinical effectiveness trial of the Newborn Behavioural Observations (NBO) for new families with maternal antenatal risk factors for postnatal depression (PND)

Methods

Design: randomised controlled trial

Participants

Study population: expectant first time single mothers and couples from the Royal Women’s Hospital and Bendigo Hospital in Australia.

Target recuitment: this project aims to randomise n=150 expectant first time mothers screening positive for antenatal risk factors of PND. The study has so far screened n=218 first time mothers during pregnancy and of these women n=84 have screened positive for antenatal risk factors of PND and have been randomised into either the G1 comparison group or G2 intervention group. The project has also recruited n=80 first time fathers and of these participants, n=35 have been included in the randomised sample as above.

Interventions

  1. Recruits with a score of less than 10 on the EPDS, less than 26 on the PASS and who do not endorse a history of depression for which they sought professional support will continue their routine visits and will be identified as G0 participants.

  2. Mothers screening positive for antenatal risk factors of PNDwill be offered referral to individual mental health support (treatment as usual) either through their respective site of recruitment (i.e., RWH or Bendigo Hospital) or in the community and randomized, with their recruited partners if relevant, into comparison group (G1) or intervention group (G2). G1 female participants will be offered treatmentfor their mental health as usual within the community or at their respective site of recruitment.

  3. G2 female participants will be offered mental health treatment as usual plus 3 NBO sessions for themselves and their partner, 1‐4 days after birth and at age 2 and 4 weeks.

Outcomes

G1 and G2 participants will complete the EPDS at 6 weeks and all three groups will complete an endpoint psychosocial questionnaire (including the EPDS and the PASS) at infant age 4 months. Additional endpoint data for G1 and G2 group participants will be collected on mother‐infant interaction, maternal depression diagnosis and infant development, using the Emotional Availability Scales, the SCID‐5 diagnostic interview depression module and the Bayley Scales of Infant Development respectively.

Starting date

10 August 2017

Contact information

Dr Susan Nicolson, Centre for Women’s Mental Health, [email protected]

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Trials register number: not registered

Nugent 2016

Trial name or title

The effect of a Newborn Behavioral Observation System‐Early Intervention (NBO‐EI) model of care

Methods

Design: this is a pragmatic trial where infants from 5 early intervention programs in one state were randomized to receive an NBO‐EI model of care or usual care for the first 12 weeks corrected gestational age. Infants were followed up to 6 months corrected gestational age and a number of maternal and infant outcomes were measured.

Participants

Thirty‐two EI‐eligible infants participated. Of the total sample, 17 received the NBO‐EI model and 22 received usual EI care.

Interventions

Intervention group: received an NBO‐EI model of care. The NBO is an 18‐item neurobehavioral relationship‐based intervention that involves a shared observation of the infant with the parent and includes elicited maneuvers with the purpose of 1) identifying infant neurobehaviors, 2) interpreting these neurobehaviors in the context of the parent‐infant interaction and infant self‐regulation and intentions, with the goal of enhancing the parent‐infant relationship, the cornerstone of optimal infant neurodevelopment and function. Specifically, the NBO‐EI group received weekly home visits from an EI provider certified in the Newborn Observational (NBO) System up to 12 weeks corrected gestational age. Content of NBO‐EI home visits consisted of intervention strategies guided by the NBO. Specifically, at each home visit the EI service provider administered the NBO with the parents and discussed 1) the infant’s attempts and successes at self‐regulation and requests for support, 2) how the infant’s neurobehaviors contribute to the parent‐infant social interactional encounter and caregiver bonding. The intervention group received the NBO‐EI during their weekly home visit for up to twelve successive weeks. The final intervention visit took place no later than the end of the twelfth week of the infant’s life, when the infant was three months of age, adjusted for prematurity. After the final study visit, in accordance with federal mandates governing EI service delivery, EI families chose to continue with weekly home visits or modified their home visit schedule.

Usual care group: the usual care (UC) group received traditional EI home‐based service delivery. The UC group received weekly home visits from an EI provider not certified in the NBO. The content of UC group home visits consisted of therapeutic and developmental activities deemed appropriate for the infant’s adjusted age and typically included visual tracking, reaching and grasping toys of a variety of textures, and tolerance of developmental play. The final study visit took place no later than the end of the twelfth week of the infant’s life, when the infant was three months of age, adjusted for prematurity. After the final study visit, in accordance with federal mandates governing EI service delivery, EI families chose to continue with weekly home visits or modified their home visit schedule.

Outcomes

Maternal depressive symptoms was collected using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES‐D). The CES‐D asks mothers to report, on a 4‐point scale (0=rarely/none of the time to 3=all of the time), their frequency of symptoms for 20 scale items. Scores of 16 or greater indicate clinically significant depressive symptoms. The CES‐D was collected, via phone interview at the 3‐month and 6‐month follow‐up time, by an independent member of the study team who is blinded to the infant’s intervention group assignment.

Parent‐infant interaction was collected using the Parent‐Child Early Relational Assessment (PCERA) . The PCERA measures parent’s and child’s affect and behavioral characteristics during a 5‐minute play observation using 29 parent items across 3 sub‐scales of parenting quality (positive affective involvement and verbalization; negative affect and behavior; and intrusiveness, insensitivity, and inconsistency) and 28 child items across 3 sub‐scales of infant emotional/behavioral regulation (positive affect, communicative, and social skills; quality of play, interest, and attentional skills; and dysregulation and irritability). For the this study, we utilized scores for the 3 parent and 3 child sub‐scales. Sub‐scale scores range from 1 (negative relational quality) to 5 (positive relational quality). PCERA scores were ascertained from a 5‐minute video clip taken by the EI home visitor (NBO‐EI and usual care groups) at 3 months and 6 months corrected gestational age. The 3‐month video will occur at the final study home visit. The 6‐month video coincided with the 6‐month eligibility / follow‐up evaluation that routinely occurs in MA EI programs. All videotapes will be reviewed and coded by an independent researcher trained in the PCERA.

Parent’s perceptions of the quality of their interaction with their infant and the extent to which their home visitor facilitated optimal interaction was measured using the Home Visiting Index.[i] Home Visiting index (HVI) is a 25‐item scale that asks parents to rate, on a 4‐point scale (1=strongly agree and 4=strongly disagree) their degree of agreement with statements about the quality of EI service delivery. At the final study visit, parents were asked to complete the HVI and return it in a self‐addressed stamped envelope to the study team.

Infant self‐regulation was collected using the Bayley Scales of Infants Development (BSID), Adaptive scale. The BSID, Adaptive Scale was collected, via phone interview at the 3‐month and 6‐month follow‐up time, by an independent member of the study team who is blinded to the infant’s intervention group assignment.

Cognitive and personal‐social, function was collected using the Battelle Developmental Inventory (BDI‐2) cognitive and personal‐social sub‐scales. Batelle scores were collected via videotaped EI eligibility evaluation (at 6 months postterm) by an independent member of the study team who is blinded to the infant’s intervention group assignment.

[i] NugentJK. Home visiting index. Brazelton Institute, Children’s Hospital, Boston; 2003

Starting date

1 November 2016

Contact information

Beth M McManus, PT, MPH, ScD, [email protected], Mobile: 617‐529‐8138

Notes

Conflicts of interests: not mentioned

Source of funding: not mentioned

Trials register number: nor registered

EPDS: Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale; NBO: Newborn Behavioral Observations system.

Data and analyses

Open in table viewer
Comparison 1. NBAS or NBO versus control

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction: postintervention Show forest plot

7

304

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

‐0.53 [‐0.90, ‐0.17]

Analysis 1.1

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction: postintervention.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction: postintervention.

2 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

Analysis 1.2

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 2 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 2 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score.

3 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score (dichotomous data) Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

Analysis 1.3

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 3 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score (dichotomous data).

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 3 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score (dichotomous data).

4 Infant social, emotional, cognitive and motor development Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

Analysis 1.4

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 4 Infant social, emotional, cognitive and motor development.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 4 Infant social, emotional, cognitive and motor development.

4.1 BSID: mental development

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

4.2 BSID: psychomotor development

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

5 Caregiver perception of infant Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

Analysis 1.5

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 5 Caregiver perception of infant.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 5 Caregiver perception of infant.

6 Caregiver knowledge of infant behaviour (multiple choice factual questions) Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

Analysis 1.6

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 6 Caregiver knowledge of infant behaviour (multiple choice factual questions).

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 6 Caregiver knowledge of infant behaviour (multiple choice factual questions).

7 Caregiver stress Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

Analysis 1.7

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 7 Caregiver stress.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 7 Caregiver stress.

7.1 PSI: parent‐related sources of stress

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

7.2 PSI: child‐related sources of stress

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

Open in table viewer
Comparison 2. NBAS or NBO versus control: subgroup analysis

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction Show forest plot

7

304

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

‐0.53 [‐0.90, ‐0.17]

Analysis 2.1

Comparison 2 NBAS or NBO versus control: subgroup analysis, Outcome 1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction.

Comparison 2 NBAS or NBO versus control: subgroup analysis, Outcome 1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction.

1.1 NBAS

5

231

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

‐0.49 [‐0.99, ‐0.00]

1.2 NBO

2

73

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

‐0.69 [‐1.18, ‐0.20]

Study flow diagram.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 1

Study flow diagram.

Risk of bias summary: review authors' judgments about each risk of bias item for each included study.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 2

Risk of bias summary: review authors' judgments about each risk of bias item for each included study.

Risk of bias graph: review authors' judgments about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 3

Risk of bias graph: review authors' judgments about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction: postintervention.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.1

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction: postintervention.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 2 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.2

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 2 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 3 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score (dichotomous data).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.3

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 3 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score (dichotomous data).

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 4 Infant social, emotional, cognitive and motor development.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.4

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 4 Infant social, emotional, cognitive and motor development.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 5 Caregiver perception of infant.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.5

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 5 Caregiver perception of infant.

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 6 Caregiver knowledge of infant behaviour (multiple choice factual questions).
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.6

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 6 Caregiver knowledge of infant behaviour (multiple choice factual questions).

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 7 Caregiver stress.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.7

Comparison 1 NBAS or NBO versus control, Outcome 7 Caregiver stress.

Comparison 2 NBAS or NBO versus control: subgroup analysis, Outcome 1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 2.1

Comparison 2 NBAS or NBO versus control: subgroup analysis, Outcome 1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction.

Summary of findings for the main comparison. Summary of findings: NBAS or NBO versus control for caregiver‐infant interaction, caregiver mental health, and caregiver functioning

NBAS or NBO versus control for caregiver‐infant interaction, caregiver mental health, and caregiver functioning

Patient or population: caregiver‐infant dyads

Settings: hospitals, clinics, home

Intervention: NBAS or NBO

Comparison: NBAS or NBO administered with no interaction

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

Number of participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Control group

NBAS or NBO

Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction (parental sensitivity, attunement, etc.); continuous data

Assessed at postintervention using validated assessment scales for caregiver‐infant interaction: higher scores indicate better outcome

The mean score for quality of caregiver‐infant interaction ranged across control groups from 3.72 to 51.90

The mean caregiver‐infant interaction score in the intervention groups was 0.53 lower (0.90 lower to 0.17 lower)

304
(7 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
Very low1

Caregiver mental health (depression), dichotomous data

Assessed at postintervention using the EPDS; lower scores indicate less depression

Low‐risk population

RR 0.23 (0.05 to 1.04)

106

(1 study)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
Low2

157 per 1000

36 per 1000

(8 to 163)

Medium‐risk population

NA

NA

High‐risk population

NA

NA

Infant social, emotional, cognitive and motor development

Assessed when infant aged 4 months, using the BSID; higher scores indicate better development

The mean score for infant mental development in the control group was 107.83

The mean score for infant mental development in the intervention groups was0.13 lower (0.48 lower to 0.22 higher)

125

(1 study)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
Low2

Caregiver perception of infant (parents' perception of the degree of difficult temperament of the infant)

Assessed at postintervention, 8 weeks after delivery; higher score indicates better outcome

The mean score for caregiver perception of infant in the control group was 18.90

The mean score for caregiver perception of the infant in the intervention group was 0.36 lower (0.95 lower to 0.24 higher)

44

(1 study)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
Low2

Caregiver stress (maternal perceptions of her adjustment to the parenting role)

Assessed when infant aged 4 months, using the PSI

The mean score for parent‐related caregiver stress in the control group was 2.19

The mean score for parent‐related caregiver stress in the intervention groups was0.00 (0.35 lower to 0.35 higher)

125

(1 study)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
Low2

Caregiver knowledge (related to infants' physical capacities, including reflexes and senses)

Assessed at postintervention, using multiple choice factual questions; higher scores indicate better outcome

The mean score for caregiver knowledge in the control groups was8.30

The mean score for caregiver knowledge in the intervention groups was 1.30 higher (0.44 to 2.16 higher)

26

(1 study)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
Very low1

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% CI) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
BSID: Bayley Scales of Infant Development; CI: confidence interval; EPDS: Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale; NA: not applicable; NBAS: Neonatal Behavioural Assessment Scale; NBO: Newborn Behavioural Observations system; PSI: Parenting Stress Index; RR: risk ratio; SMD: standardised mean difference.

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: we are very uncertain about the estimate.

1Downgraded due to risk of bias due to poor quality research (e.g. limitations in design, including inadequate randomisation or allocation procedures, together with attrition, which ranged from 4% to 20% in seven studies – none of which conducted an intention‐to‐treat analysis), inconsistency for the main outcome due to high levels of heterogeneity for the NBAS; and indirectness in terms of the low levels of generalisability to wider risk groups within the population.

2Downgraded due to risk of bias due to poor quality research (e.g. limitations in design, including inadequate randomisation or allocation procedures, together with attrition, which ranged from 4% to 20% in seven studies – none of which conducted an intention‐to‐treat analysis), and indirectness in terms of the low levels of generalisability to wider risk groups within the population.

Figuras y tablas -
Summary of findings for the main comparison. Summary of findings: NBAS or NBO versus control for caregiver‐infant interaction, caregiver mental health, and caregiver functioning
Table 1. Unused methods table

Unit of analysis issues

Cluster‐randomised trials

The randomisation of clusters can result in an overestimate of the precision of the results (with a higher risk of a type I error) when their use has not been compensated for in the analysis. Had we included a cluster‐RCT, we planned to explore whether the authors had adequately controlled for the effects of clustering in the study. When they had, and when there was little difference between the study designs, and when there was unlikely to be an interaction between the effect of the intervention and the choice of randomisation method, we planned to combine the data from the cluster‐RCT with data from individual RCTs. When the effects of clustering had not been controlled for properly, we planned to derive an estimate of the intracluster correlation coefficient (ICC) from the study or that of a similar population, and to report whether an ICC had been used and conduct sensitivity analyses to determine the effect of using an ICC. We also planned to assess the impact of including data from a cluster‐RCT on the inclusion of the study in the meta‐analyses using a sensitivity analysis to explore the effects of the randomisation method. However, no cluster RCTs were identified or included.

Trials with multiple treatments groups

In the event that we had identified a multi‐arm study in which the NBAS and NBO had been compared with an alternative treatment and a control group, we planned to only extract data from two arms (e.g. NBAS and control group). In the event that we had identified a multi‐arm study in which the NBAS had been compared with the NBO and involved only one control group, we planned to combine the data from the NBAS and NBO arms for primary analyses and to conduct secondary, subgroup analyses and split the control group data. However, we identified no multiple treatment groups.

Cross‐over trials

Cross‐over trials are not possible with this type of intervention, and none were identified.

Assessment of reporting bias

We planned to draw funnel plots (estimated differences in treatment effects against their standard error) if there was a sufficient number of included studies (e.g. more than 10), to identify asymmetry due to publication bias and other small study effects. We also planned to assess whether there had been selective reporting of outcomes and to assess the impact of this using a sensitivity analysis. However, there were insufficient studies to undertake this analysis.

Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity

We planned to explore possible reasons for heterogeneity by undertaking the following, additional subgroup analyses, scrutinising studies to determine the extent of between‐trial differences.

  1. Level of vulnerability of infant and caregiver (e.g. caregiver age, family socioeconomic status, infant health, etc.).

  2. Caregiver parity (whether first‐time or experienced caregivers).

  3. Whether mothers only, fathers only, or both caregivers are present at the session.

  4. Whether single or multiple sessions were conducted.

  5. Setting in which the sessions were held.

However, it was only possible to undertake subgroup analysis for NBAS versus NBO, due to the small number of studies.

Sensitivity analysis

We planned to conduct sensitivity analyses on the basis of method of sequence generation only, to assess the robustness of the results, but this was not possible due to the small number of studies.

NBAS: Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale; NBO: Newborn Behavioral Observations system; RCT: randomised controlled trial.

Figuras y tablas -
Table 1. Unused methods table
Comparison 1. NBAS or NBO versus control

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction: postintervention Show forest plot

7

304

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

‐0.53 [‐0.90, ‐0.17]

2 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

3 Caregiver mental health (maternal depression): EPDS postintervention score (dichotomous data) Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

4 Infant social, emotional, cognitive and motor development Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

4.1 BSID: mental development

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

4.2 BSID: psychomotor development

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

5 Caregiver perception of infant Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

6 Caregiver knowledge of infant behaviour (multiple choice factual questions) Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

7 Caregiver stress Show forest plot

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Totals not selected

7.1 PSI: parent‐related sources of stress

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

7.2 PSI: child‐related sources of stress

1

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

Figuras y tablas -
Comparison 1. NBAS or NBO versus control
Comparison 2. NBAS or NBO versus control: subgroup analysis

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Quality of caregiver‐infant interaction Show forest plot

7

304

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

‐0.53 [‐0.90, ‐0.17]

1.1 NBAS

5

231

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

‐0.49 [‐0.99, ‐0.00]

1.2 NBO

2

73

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

‐0.69 [‐1.18, ‐0.20]

Figuras y tablas -
Comparison 2. NBAS or NBO versus control: subgroup analysis