Scolaris Content Display Scolaris Content Display

Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews

Tamaño de la porción, el envase o la vajilla para el cambio de selección y consumo de alimentos, bebidas alcohólicas y tabaco

Información

DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD011045.pub2Copiar DOI
Base de datos:
  1. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
Versión publicada:
  1. 14 septiembre 2015see what's new
Tipo:
  1. Intervention
Etapa:
  1. Review
Grupo Editorial Cochrane:
  1. Grupo Cochrane de Salud pública

Copyright:
  1. Copyright © 2018 The Authors. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. on behalf of The Cochrane Collaboration.
  2. This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Licence, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Cifras del artículo

Altmetric:

Citado por:

Citado 0 veces por enlace Crossref Cited-by

Contraer

Autores

  • Gareth J Hollandsa

    Correspondencia a: Behaviour and Health Research Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

    [email protected]

    GH and IS contributed equally to this work

  • Ian Shemilta

    EPPI‐Centre, University College London, London, UK

    GH and IS contributed equally to this work

  • Theresa M Marteau

    Behaviour and Health Research Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

  • Susan A Jebb

    Nuffield Department of Primary Care Health Sciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK

  • Hannah B Lewis

    MRC Human Nutrition Research, Cambridge, UK

  • Yinghui Wei

    Centre for Mathematical Sciences, School of Computing, Electronics and Mathematics, University of Plymouth, Plymouth, UK

  • Julian P T Higgins

    Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK

  • David Ogilvie

    MRC Epidemiology Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Contributions of authors

Draft the protocol ‐ all authors

Develop a search strategy ‐ GJH, IS

Search for trials ‐ GJH, IS

Obtain copies of trials ‐ GJH, IS

Select which studies to include ‐ GJH, IS, DO

Extract data from studies ‐ GJH, IS, HBL, YW, JPTH

Enter data into RevMan ‐ GJH, IS

Carry out the analysis ‐ YW, JPTH, IS, GJH

Interpret the analysis ‐ all authors

Draft the final review ‐ all authors

Sources of support

Internal sources

  • Kings College London, UK.

    Database access

  • University of Cambridge, UK.

    Computer provision, database access

  • University of East Anglia, UK.

    Database access

  • University of Bristol, UK.

    Computer provision

  • Plymouth University, UK.

    Computer provision

External sources

  • Funded by UK Department of Health Policy Research Programme (107/0001‐ Policy Research Unit in Behaviour and Health), UK.

  • YW was supported by the UK Medical Research Council (MRC) grant to the MRC Clinical Trials Unit Hub for Trials Methodology Research [Grant number MSA7355QP21], UK.

Declarations of interest

Gareth Hollands declares no financial or other conflicts of interest.

Ian Shemilt declares no financial or other conflicts of interest.

Theresa Marteau declares no financial or other conflicts of interest.

Susan Jebb is Chair of the Public Health Responsibility Deal Food Network, which develops voluntary agreements with industry to improve health, including reductions in portion size of foods high in fat, saturated fat, sugar and salt. She has also led research projects in which foods have been provided by a range of commercial companies as part of dietary intervention studies funded by public bodies. She was also a co‐author of a published study (completed 2010) funded by the Coca‐Cola Institute for Health & Wellness, which showed no effect on weight loss of a putative functional beverage.

Hannah Lewis declares no financial or other conflicts of interest.

Yinghui Wei declares no financial or other conflicts of interest.

Julian Higgins declares no financial or other conflicts of interest.

David Ogilvie declares no financial or other conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgements

Production of this Cochrane review was funded by the UK Department of Health Policy Research Programme (107/0001‐Policy Research Unit in Behaviour and Health). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the UK Department of Health. We would like to acknowledge the contributions of Julie Glanville (York Health Economics Consortium, University of York, UK) who reviewed a draft of our MEDLINE search strategy, Claire Stansfield (EPPI‐Centre, University of London, UK) who helped to develop and ran our searches of the TRoPHI database, and Helen Morgan (TSC, Cochrane Public Health Review Group) who initially reviewed our overall search strategy. We would like to thank Jodie Doyle (Managing Editor), Liz Waters (Co‐ordinating Editor) and their colleagues in the Cochrane Public Health Review Group ‐ especially Jonathan Shepherd (our Contact Editor), Anke Rowher (Methods Advisor), Ruth Turley (Author) and Daniel Francis (Editor). We also greatly appreciated input from Sadequa Shahrook (Research Fellow, Department of Health Policy ,National Center for Child Health and Development, Tokyo, Japan) and anonymous external referees who provided helpful comments on the penultimate and final draft full review, as well as other editors, peer reviewers and critical friends who have provided helpful feedback at various stages. Finally, we would like to acknowledge all the authors of included studies who responded to our requests for clarifications or data ‐ especially those who responded with respect to several included studies.

Version history

Published

Title

Stage

Authors

Version

2015 Sep 14

Portion, package or tableware size for changing selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco

Review

Gareth J Hollands, Ian Shemilt, Theresa M Marteau, Susan A Jebb, Hannah B Lewis, Yinghui Wei, Julian P T Higgins, David Ogilvie

https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD011045.pub2

2014 Apr 01

Portion, package or tableware size for changing selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco

Protocol

Gareth J Hollands, Ian Shemilt, Theresa M Marteau, Susan A Jebb, Hannah B Lewis, Yinghui Wei, Julian PT Higgins, David Ogilvie

https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD011045

Differences between protocol and review

A difference between the protocol (Hollands 2014) and review is that the proposed search of the Cochrane Public Health Group Specialised Register was not, in practice, conducted. This omission is unlikely to have had any impact on the review. Study records on the Cochrane Public Health Group Specialised Register are submitted for inclusion in the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL) on a quarterly basis and we conducted searches of CENTRAL for this review up to 30 January 2015. Also, at the protocol stage, we intended to use the most commonly available measure of participants' socioeconomic status to construct the socioeconomic status context variable (see Data extraction and management). We were unable to do this in practice because no single proxy measure of participants' socioeconomic status, such as education or income, was commonly measured in and reported by included studies. Therefore we instead coded a binary study‐level covariate based on authors' explicit descriptors of the study sample and/or setting (e.g. "Low income Hispanic or non‐Hispanic African American children and their mothers", or "Faculty, graduate students, and staff members of the Department of Food Science and Nutritional Science of a large Midwestern university". Unless explicitly described as being of low socioeconomic status, we coded the context of included studies as high socioeconomic status.

Notes

From the author team, 10 October, 2018, in response to recent retraction of several studies by Brian Wansink

On the 19th September 2018, JAMA, JAMA Internal Medicine and JAMA Pediatrics retracted six articles on which Brian Wansink (John Dyson Professor of Marketing at Cornell University), was an author (https://media.jamanetwork.com/news‐item/jama‐network‐retracts‐6‐articles‐that‐included‐dr‐brian‐wansink‐as‐author/). Given seven previous retractions, this means that 13 of his articles have been retracted as of 10th October 2018 (http://retractiondatabase.org/RetractionSearch.aspx#?auth%3dWansink). The retracted articles are listed at the end of this note.

None of the 13 retracted articles authored by Wansink were included in this Cochrane review (one was excluded due to ineligible study design). The results and conclusions of the review are therefore not affected.

Other articles on which Wansink is an author, and which have not been retracted, were included in this review. It includes 72 studies, of which 13 studies were authored by Wansink.

The effects reported in this review are uncertain, attributable in part to evidence that is at significant risk of bias with, at best, GRADE ratings of ‘moderate’ (meaning that further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in estimated effects). These retractions do, however, introduce additional uncertainty regarding the veracity of other studies Wansink has authored, including those contributing to this review. Should any study included in this review be retracted, we will withdraw that study’s data from updated meta‐analyses conducted as part of future updates of this Cochrane review.

Gareth Hollands and Theresa Marteau, on behalf of the author team

Retracted studies (as of 10th October 2018)

Wansink B, Tal A, Shimizu M (2012). First foods most: after 18‐hour fast, people drawn to starches first and vegetables last. Arch Intern Med. 172(12): 961‐963.

Tal A, Wansink B (2013). Fattening fasting: hungry grocery shoppers buy more calories, not more food. JAMA Intern Med. 173(12): 1146‐1148.

Tal A, Zuckerman S, Wansink B (2014). Watch what you eat: action‐related television content increases food intake. JAMA Intern Med. 174(11): 1842‐1843.

Wansink B, Cheney MM (2005). Super Bowls: serving bowl size and food consumption. JAMA. 293(14): 1727‐1728.

Wansink B, Payne C, Werle C (2008). Consequences of belonging to the “clean plate club”. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 162(10): 994‐995.

Hanks AS, Just DR, Wansink B (2013). Preordering school lunch encourages better food choices by children. JAMA Pediatr. 167(7): 673‐674.

Vuorinen A‐L, Strahilevitz MA, Wansink B, Safer DL (2017). Shifts in the Enjoyment of Healthy and Unhealthy Behaviors Affect Short‐ and Long‐Term Postbariatric Weight Loss. Bariatric Surgical Practice and Patient Care. 12(1): 35–42.

Wansink B, Just DR, Payne CR, Klinger MZ (2012). Attractive names sustain increased vegetable intake in schools. Prev Med. 55(4):330‐332.

Wansink B, Westgren R (2003). Profiling taste‐motivated segments. Appetite. 41(3): 323‐7.

Sigirci O, Rockmore M, Wansink B (2016). How Traumatic Violence Permanently Changes Shopping Behavior. Front. Psychol. 7:1298.

Sigirci O, Wansink B (2015). Low prices and high regret: how pricing influences regret at all‐you‐can‐eat buffets. BMC Nutrition 1:36.

Wansink B, Park S‐B (2002). Sensory Suggestiveness and Labeling: Do Soy Labels Bias Taste? Journal of Sensory Studies. 17(5): 483‐491.

Wansink B, Just DR, Payne CR (2012). Can Branding Improve School Lunches? Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 166(10): 967‐968.

Keywords

MeSH

PICO

Population
Intervention
Comparison
Outcome

El uso y la enseñanza del modelo PICO están muy extendidos en el ámbito de la atención sanitaria basada en la evidencia para formular preguntas y estrategias de búsqueda y para caracterizar estudios o metanálisis clínicos. PICO son las siglas en inglés de cuatro posibles componentes de una pregunta de investigación: paciente, población o problema; intervención; comparación; desenlace (outcome).

Para saber más sobre el uso del modelo PICO, puede consultar el Manual Cochrane.

Final conceptual model. The 28 constructs included in the provisional conceptual model (Hollands 2014) and retained in this final version are shown in plain type. The 22 constructs added to this final conceptual model based on theory and evidence encountered during the review process are shown in red type. The 2 constructs included in the provisional conceptual model (Hollands 2014) but excluded from this final version are shown in strikethrough plain type. See for a full record of the conceptual model development process.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 1

Final conceptual model. The 28 constructs included in the provisional conceptual model (Hollands 2014) and retained in this final version are shown in plain type. The 22 constructs added to this final conceptual model based on theory and evidence encountered during the review process are shown in red type. The 2 constructs included in the provisional conceptual model (Hollands 2014) but excluded from this final version are shown in strikethrough plain type. See Table 1 for a full record of the conceptual model development process.

PRISMA study flow diagram.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 2

PRISMA study flow diagram.

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all eligible studies (N = 83. 'Risk of bias' assessments completed for 72 eligible studies included in the review. White spaces in the bars of this graph denote the respective proportions of the 72 included studies that did not measure (i) selection or (ii) consumption outcomes. See also Results of the search and ).
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 3

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all eligible studies (N = 83. 'Risk of bias' assessments completed for 72 eligible studies included in the review. White spaces in the bars of this graph denote the respective proportions of the 72 included studies that did not measure (i) selection or (ii) consumption outcomes. See also Results of the search and Figure 2).

Effect sizes re‐expressed using familiar metrics
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 4

Effect sizes re‐expressed using familiar metrics

Forest plot of the standardised mean difference in unregulated consumption of food or tobacco between participants exposed to larger (intervention) versus smaller (control) sized portions, packages, individual units and/or tableware
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 5

Forest plot of the standardised mean difference in unregulated consumption of food or tobacco between participants exposed to larger (intervention) versus smaller (control) sized portions, packages, individual units and/or tableware

Assessing publication bias. Funnel plots including all studies reporting the selection outcome (left) and consumption outcome (right) do not show asymmetry (Egger test P value = 0.20 and P value = 0.18 respectively)
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 6

Assessing publication bias. Funnel plots including all studies reporting the selection outcome (left) and consumption outcome (right) do not show asymmetry (Egger test P value = 0.20 and P value = 0.18 respectively)

Summary effect sizes (standardised mean differences) in subgroups of studies (consumption outcome)
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 7

Summary effect sizes (standardised mean differences) in subgroups of studies (consumption outcome)

Bubble plots. Fitted meta‐regression lines showing associations between study‐level effect sizes for consumption and study characteristics (continuous variables) identified as effect modifiers: a) FSA score; b) energy density; c) age.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 8

Bubble plots. Fitted meta‐regression lines showing associations between study‐level effect sizes for consumption and study characteristics (continuous variables) identified as effect modifiers: a) FSA score; b) energy density; c) age.

Forest plot of the standardised mean difference in unregulated selection (without purchase) of food between participants exposed to larger (intervention) versus smaller (control) sized portions, packages and/or tableware
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 9

Forest plot of the standardised mean difference in unregulated selection (without purchase) of food between participants exposed to larger (intervention) versus smaller (control) sized portions, packages and/or tableware

Summary effect sizes (standardised mean differences) in subgroups of studies (selection outcome)
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 10

Summary effect sizes (standardised mean differences) in subgroups of studies (selection outcome)

Forest plot of the standardised mean difference in unregulated selection without purchase of fruit juices or water between participants exposed to shorter, wider (intervention) versus taller, narrower (control) empty glasses or plastic bottles
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 11

Forest plot of the standardised mean difference in unregulated selection without purchase of fruit juices or water between participants exposed to shorter, wider (intervention) versus taller, narrower (control) empty glasses or plastic bottles

Summary of findings for the main comparison. Food: Larger versus smaller‐sized portions, packages or tableware for changing quantity consumed or selected

Food: Larger versus smaller‐sized portions, packages or tableware for changing quantity consumed or selected

Population: children and adults
Settings: high‐income countries, laboratory and field settings
Intervention: larger‐sized portion, package, individual unit or item of tableware
Comparison: smaller‐sized portion, package, individual unit or item of tableware

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Smaller‐sized portion, package, individual unit or item of tableware

Larger‐sized portion, package, individual unit or item of tableware

Consumption

Mean daily energy intake from food among a representative sample of UK children and adults is 1689 kcal3

Mean daily energy intake from food would be 189 kcal (11.2%) higher with the intervention (144 to 228 kcal higher) among UK children and adults

Mean consumption in the intervention group was 0.38 standard deviations higher (0.29 higher to 0.46 higher)

6603
(86 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊕⊝
MODERATE 1

‐ Consumption among children

Mean daily energy intake from food among a representative sample of UK children is 1651 kcal3

Mean daily energy intake from food would be 95 kcal (5.7%) higher with the intervention (45 to 140 kcal higher) among UK children

Mean consumption in the intervention group was 0.21 standard deviations higher (0.1 higher to 0.31 higher)

1421
(22 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊕⊝
MODERATE 1

‐ Consumption among adults

Mean daily energy intake from food among a representative sample of UK adults is 1727 kcal3

Mean daily energy intake from food would be 247 kcal (14.3%) higher with the intervention (215 to 279 kcal higher) among UK adults

Mean consumption in the intervention group was 0.46 standard deviations higher (0.40 higher to 0.52 higher)

5182
(64 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊕⊝
MODERATE 1

Selection without purchase

Mean daily energy intake from food among a representative sample of UK children and adults is 1689 kcal3

Mean daily energy intake from food would be 209 kcal (12.4%) higher with the intervention (119 to 293 kcal higher) among UK children and adults4

Mean selection without purchase in the intervention group was 0.42 standard deviations higher (0.24 higher to 0.59 higher)

1164
(13 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊕⊝
MODERATE 1

‐ Selection without purchase among children

Mean daily energy intake from food among a representative sample of UK children is 1651 kcal3

Mean daily energy intake from food would be 63 kcal (3.8%) higher with the intervention (27 to 153 kcal higher) among UK children4

Mean selection without purchase in the intervention group was 0.14 standard deviations higher (0.06 lower to 0.34 higher)

382
(4 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
LOW 1,2

‐ Selection without purchase among adults

Mean daily energy intake from food among a representative sample of UK adults is 1727 kcal3

Mean daily energy intake from food would be 188 kcal (10.9%) higher with the intervention (188 to 403 kcal higher) among UK adults4

Mean selection without purchase in the intervention group was 0.55 standard deviations higher (0.35 higher to 0.75 higher)

782
(9 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊕⊝
MODERATE 1

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in representative UK samples3 and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: confidence interval

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1Rated down by one level for study limitations: we assessed risk of bias as unclear or high in all incorporated studies.

2Rated down by one level for imprecision: number of participants (effective sample size) incorporated into analysis is less than the number of patients generated by a conventional sample size calculation for a single adequately powered trial (optimal information size) and the confidence interval crosses zero.

3Estimates of means and standard deviations based on an unweighted analysis of data from the UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey, Years 1‐4 (National Centre for Social Research 2012) ‐ see Data synthesis.

4Illustration of equivalent absolute effect on daily energy intake from food assumes that all foods selected are consumed.

Figuras y tablas -
Summary of findings for the main comparison. Food: Larger versus smaller‐sized portions, packages or tableware for changing quantity consumed or selected
Summary of findings 2. Alcohol: Larger versus smaller‐sized portions, packages or tableware for changing quantity consumed or selected

Alcohol: Larger versus smaller‐sized portions, packages or tableware for changing quantity consumed or selected

Population: children and adults
Settings: high‐income countries, laboratory and field settings
Intervention: larger‐sized portion, package, individual unit or item of tableware
Comparison: smaller‐sized portion, package, individual unit or item of tableware

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Smaller‐sized portion, package, individual unit or item of tableware

Larger‐sized portion, package, individual unit or item of tableware

Consumption

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

‐ Consumption among children

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

‐ Consumption among adults

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

Selection with or without purchase

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

‐ Selection with or without purchase among children

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

‐ Selection with or without purchase among adults

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: confidence interval

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

Figuras y tablas -
Summary of findings 2. Alcohol: Larger versus smaller‐sized portions, packages or tableware for changing quantity consumed or selected
Summary of findings 3. Tobacco: Longer versus shorter cigarettes for changing quantity consumed or selected

Tobacco: Longer versus shorter cigarettes for changing quantity consumed or selected

Population: children and adults
Settings: high‐income countries, laboratory settings
Intervention: longer cigarettes
Comparison: shorter cigarettes

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Shorter cigarettes

Longer cigarettes

Consumption

Mean number of cigarettes smoked per day among a representative sample of UK adults is 13

Mean number of cigarettes smoked per day would be 2 higher with the intervention (1 to 5 higher) among UK adults

Mean consumption in the intervention group was 0.25 standard deviations higher (0.14 lower to 0.65 higher)

108
(6 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
LOW 1,2

‐ Consumption among children

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

‐ Consumption among adults

Mean number of cigarettes smoked per day among a representative sample of UK adults is 13

Mean number of cigarettes smoked per day would be 2 higher with the intervention (1 to 5 higher) among UK adults

Mean consumption in the intervention group was 0.25 standard deviations higher (0.14 lower to 0.65 higher)

108
(6 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
LOW 1,2

Selection with or without purchase

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

‐ Selection with or without purchase among children

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

‐ Selection with or without purchase among adults

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: confidence interval

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1Rated down by one level for study limitations: we assessed risk of bias as unclear or high in all incorporated studies.

2Rated down by one level for imprecision: number of participants (effective sample size) incorporated into analysis is less than the number of patients generated by a conventional sample size calculation for a single adequately powered trial (optimal information size) and confidence interval crosses zero.

3Estimates of means and standard deviations based on an unweighted analysis of data from the UK Opinions and Lifestyle Survey, 2012 (Office for National Statistics 2012) ‐ see Data synthesis.

Figuras y tablas -
Summary of findings 3. Tobacco: Longer versus shorter cigarettes for changing quantity consumed or selected
Summary of findings 4. Food: Shorter, wider versus taller, narrower glasses or plastic bottles (shape) for changing quantity of non‐alcoholic beverages consumed or selected

Shorter, wider versus taller, narrower glasses or plastic bottles (shape) for changing quantity of non‐alcoholic beverages consumed or selected

Patient or population: children and adults
Settings: high‐income countries, field settings
Intervention: shorter, wider glasses or plastic bottles
Comparison: taller, narrower glasses or plastic bottles

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Shorter, wider glasses or plastic bottles

Taller, narrower glasses or plastic bottles

Consumption

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve among a representative sample of UK adults is 245 grams8

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve would be 195 grams (79.6%) higher with the intervention (95 to 296 grams higher) among UK adults

Mean consumption in the intervention group was 1.17 standard deviations higher (0.57 higher to 1.78 higher)

50
(1 independent comparison)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
VERY LOW 1,2

‐ Consumption among adults

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve among a representative sample of UK adults is 245 grams8

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve would be 195 grams (79.6%) higher with the intervention (95 to 296 grams higher) among UK adults

Mean consumption in the intervention group was 1.17 standard deviations higher (0.57 higher to 1.78 higher)

50
(1 independent comparison)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
VERY LOW 1,2

‐ Consumption among children

No evidence is available

(0 independent comparisons)

Selection without purchase

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve among a representative sample of UK children and adults is 234 grams8

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve would be 242 grams (103.4%) higher with the intervention (86 to 400 grams higher) among UK children and adults9

Mean selection without purchase in the intervention group was 1.47 standard deviations higher (0.52 higher to 2.43 higher)

232
(3 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
LOW 3,4

‐ Selection without purchase among children

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve among a representative sample of UK children is 228 grams8

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve would be 377 grams (165.5%) higher with the intervention (292 to 462 grams higher) among UK children9

Mean selection without purchase in the intervention group was 2.31 standard deviations higher (1.79 higher to 2.83 higher)

96
(1 independent comparison)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
LOW 5, 6

‐ Selection without purchase among adults

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve among a representative sample of UK adults is 245 grams8

Mean quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in a single serve would be 171 grams (70.1%) higher with the intervention (68 to 274 grams higher) among UK adults9

Mean selection without purchase in the intervention group was 1.03 standard deviations higher (0.41 higher to 1.65 higher)

136
(2 independent comparisons)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
LOW 3,7

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: confidence interval

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1Rated down two levels for study limitations: study assessed at high risk of bias with respect to the consumption outcome (see Characteristics of included studies 'Risk of bias' tables).

2Rated down one level for imprecision: number of participants (effective sample size) incorporated into analysis is less than the number of patients generated by a conventional sample size calculation for a single adequately powered trial (optimal information size) based on the lower limit of the confidence interval.

3Rated down one level for study limitations: studies assessed at unclear or high risk of bias with respect to the selection outcome (see Characteristics of included studies 'Risk of bias' tables).

4Rated down one level for inconsistency. I2 statistic from the random‐effects model shows that 90.1% of the total variance in study‐level estimates of this effect was due to statistical heterogeneity.

5Rated down one level for study limitations: study assessed at unclear risk of bias with respect to the selection outcome (see Characteristics of included studies 'Risk of bias' tables).

6Rated down one level for imprecision: single study.

7Rated down one level for inconsistency: point estimates are dissimilar and confidence intervals do not overlap.

8Estimates of means and standard deviations based on an unweighted analysis of data from the UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey, Years 1‐4 (National Centre for Social Research 2012) ‐ see Data synthesis.

9Illustration of equivalent absolute effect on quantity of energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverages consumed in single serve assumes that all energy‐containing non‐alcoholic beverage selected in a single serve is consumed.

Figuras y tablas -
Summary of findings 4. Food: Shorter, wider versus taller, narrower glasses or plastic bottles (shape) for changing quantity of non‐alcoholic beverages consumed or selected
Table 1. Record of conceptual model development

Construct

Variable description (type)

Category

Included in provisional conceptual model?

Included in final conceptual model?

Included study first encountered

Other included studies encountered

Rationale for inclusion in final conceptual model

Supporting evidence

Rationale for exclusion from final conceptual model

Study design

Randomised controlled trial or cluster‐randomised controlled trial (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Study design

Between subjects or within‐subjects design (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Study/ intervention setting

Laboratory or field setting (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Study/ intervention setting

Selecting/consuming alone or selecting/consuming with others

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Product type

Food or tobacco (or alcohol ‐ no studies) (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Healthiness of manipulated product(s) (food products only)

FSA Nutrient Profile Score

(Continuous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Basis for calculating healthiness of manipulated product(s) (food products only)

Specific product or product category (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Energy density of manipulated product(s) (food products only)

Energy density points from FSA Nutrient Profile model (Continuous)

Study characteristic

No

Yes

Devitt 2004 (study includes concurrent manipulation of energy density)

Kral 2004a, Fisher 2007b, Leahy 2008, Looney 2011, Rolls 2006b (studies include concurrent manipulation of energy density)

Evidence from previous studies that the energy density of food can exert independent and combined influences on energy intake suggests that this has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Kral 2004a, Kral 2004b,

Rolls 2009, Bell 1998, Rolls 1999, Rolls 2006b

N/A

Target of manipulation

Portion, package, individual unit, package with individual unit, or tableware (Categorical, nominal)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Type of manipulation

Size (including volume) or shape manipulation (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Post‐hoc decision taken to conduct separate meta‐analyses for size and shape since comparisons of size were not judged conceptually comparable to comparisons of shape among the set of studies included in this review Therefore no longer conceptualised as a potential effect modifier

Manipulation from a standard size

No or yes (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

No

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

In practice it was rarely possible to code this variable based on information in study reports, and not judged practicable to code with reference to data from external sources

If applicable, direction of the change relative to standard size

Smaller or larger

(Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

No

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

In practice it was rarely possible to code this variable based on information in study reports, and not judged practicable to code with reference to data from external sources

Selection without purchasing or selection with purchasing

Selection without purchasing or selection with purchasing (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Duration of exposure to the intervention

≤ 1 day or > 1 day (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

No

Yes

N/A – Added based on discussion of collected data between 2 review authors (GJH and IS, April 2014), which identified duration of exposure as a variant characteristic of included studies (in addition to timing of outcome measurement, which had been included in our provisional conceptual model)

N/A

Duration of exposure to larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware has the potential to modify any effects of such exposure on the selection and consumption of food

Rolls 2006a, Rolls 2007a

N/A

Relationship between manipulated product(s) and consumption/ selection outcomes (food products only)

The manipulated foods comprise all of those in the study and all are selected or consumed ad libitum (Dummy)

Study characteristic

No

Yes

N/A – Added based on discussion of collected data between 2 review authors (GJH and IS, April 2014), which identified duration of exposure as a variant characteristic of included studies

N/A

This relationship may have the potential to modify any effects of such exposure on the selection and consumption of food. This is because providing any additional foods for consumption beyond those manipulated may result in additional energy consumption in either or both conditions. Given potential ceiling effects on total consumption, this could modify any intervention effect

N/A

Relationship between manipulated product(s) and consumption/ selection outcomes (food products only)

The manipulated foods are only a subset of all the foods in the study and there are other non‐manipulated foods that are compulsory to select or consume (Dummy)

Study characteristic

No

Yes

N/A – Added based on discussion of collected data between 2 review authors (GJH and IS, April 2014), which identified duration of exposure as a variant characteristic of included studies

N/A

This relationship may have the potential to modify any effects of such exposure on the selection and consumption of food. This is because providing compulsory additional foods beyond those manipulated would result in additional energy consumption in both conditions. Given potential ceiling effects on total consumption, this could attenuate any intervention effect

N/A

Relationship between manipulated product(s) and consumption/ selection outcomes (food products only)

The manipulated foods are only a subset of all the foods in the study and there are other non‐manipulated foods in study that are selected or consumed ad libitum (Dummy)

Study characteristic

No

Yes

N/A – Added based on discussion of collected data between 2 review authors (GJH and IS, April 2014), which identified duration of exposure as a variant characteristic of included studies

N/A

This relationship may have the potential to modify any effects of such exposure on the selection and consumption of food. This is because providing additional foods to be consumed ad libitum beyond those manipulated may result in additional energy consumption in either or both conditions. Given potential ceiling effects on total consumption, this could modify any intervention effect

N/A

Relationship between manipulated product(s) and consumption/ selection outcomes (food products only)

Outcome data maps directly onto the manipulated food(s) (as opposed to a wider set of foods, including but not limited to manipulated food(s)) (Dummy)

Study characteristic

No

Yes

N/A – Added based on discussion of collected data between 2 review authors (GJH and IS, April 2014), which identified duration of exposure as a variant characteristic of included studies

N/A

This relationship may have the potential to modify any effects of such exposure on the selection and consumption of food. This is because including any additional foods in outcome measurement beyond those manipulated may result in additional energy consumption being measured in either or both conditions

N/A

Concurrent intervention component(s)

Absent or present (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Socio‐economic status context

Low deprivation or high deprivation (Categorical, dichotomous)

Study characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Magnitude of the absolute difference in size

Difference between larger size and smaller size in grams

(Continuous)

Intervention characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Magnitude of the relative difference in size

Larger size expressed as a proportion (%) of smaller size (Continuous)

Intervention characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Age

Average (mean) age in years among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Gender

Proportion (%) of study completers who were female (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Ethnicity

Proportion (%) of study completers of white ethnicity (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Body mass index (BMI)

Average (mean) BMI among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Body mass index (BMI)

Average (mean) BMI‐z score among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Body weight

Average (mean) weight in kilograms among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Body weight status

Average (mean) percentage (%) body fat among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Body weight status

Proportion (%) of study completers who were overweight (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Body weight status

Proportion (%) of study completers who were obese (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Body weight status

Proportion (%) of study completers who were overweight or obese (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: dietary restraint

Average (mean) dietary restraint score among study completers ‐ Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (Stunkard 1985) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: dietary restraint

Average (mean) dietary restraint score among study completers ‐ Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (Van Strien 1986) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: dietary restraint

Average (mean) dietary restraint score among study completers ‐ Restraint Scale (Herman 1980) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: dietary disinhibition

Average (mean) dietary disinhibition score among study completers ‐ Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (Stunkard 1985) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: dietary disinhibition

Average (mean) dietary disinhibition score among study completers ‐ Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (Van Strien 1986) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: external eating

Average (mean) external eating score among study completers ‐ Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (Van Strien 1986) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Hermans 2012

Kelly 2009,

Kral 2004a

External eating (which measures the tendency to eat in response to external food‐related cues such as the sight, taste, and smell of attractive food) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Herman 2008, Burton 2007, Rodin 1981

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: emotional eating

Average (mean) emotional eating score among study completers ‐ Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (Van Strien 1986) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Kelly 2009

Kral 2004a

Emotional eating (which measures the tendency to eat in response to emotions such as anxiety, disappointment or boredom) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Van Strien 1986, Wallis 2009

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: susceptibility to hunger

Average (mean) hunger score among study completers – Three factor eating questionnaire (Stunkard 1985) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Flood 2006

Kral 2004a, Rolls 2002, Rolls 2004a, Rolls 2004b, Rolls 2006a, Rolls 2006b, Rolls 2007a, Rolls 2007b (S1), Rolls 2007b (S2), Rolls 2007b (S3), Rolls 2010a (E1), Rolls 2010b (E2)

Susceptibility to hunger (predisposition to feelings of hunger) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Provencher 2003, Lindroos 1997

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: plate cleaning tendency

Average (mean) plate cleaning tendency score among study completers ‐ 7‐point agreement scale anchored (‐3) strongly disagree and (+3) strongly agree (Marchiori 2012a, Wansink 2005e) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Marchiori 2012a

Plate cleaning tendency (the tendency for a person to consume all the food presented to them) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Wansink 2005e

N/A

Behavioural characteristic: plate cleaning tendency

Behavioural characteristic ‐ Proportion (%) of adult study completers who often or always clean the plate (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Rolls 2004a

Plate cleaning tendency (the tendency for a person to consume all the food presented to them) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Wansink 2005e

N/A

Behavioural characteristic: plate cleaning tendency

Behavioural characteristic ‐ Proportion (%) of child study completers who often or always clean the plate (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Rolls 2004a

Plate cleaning tendency (the tendency for a person to consume all the food presented to them) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Wansink 2005e

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: consumption monitoring

Average (mean) consumption monitoring score among study completers ‐ 7‐point agreement scale anchored (‐3) strongly disagree and (+3) strongly agree (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Marchiori 2012a

Consumption monitoring (the tendency for a person to pay attention to and monitor the food they are consuming) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Polivy 1986

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: binge eating

Average (mean) binge eating score among study completers ‐ Eating Disorders Examination (Fairburn 1993) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Marchiori 2012a

Binge eating (discrete episodes of eating during which the amount consumed is unusually large and there is a sense of loss of control over eating at the time) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Fairburn 1993

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: binge eating

Average (mean) binge eating score among study completers – Binge Eating Questionnaire (Gormally 1982) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Stroebele 2009

Binge eating (discrete episodes of eating during which the amount consumed is unusually large and there is a sense of loss of control over eating at the time (Fairburn 1993)) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Fairburn 1993, Cooper 2003

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: dieting behaviour

Average (mean) dieting behavior score – Eating Attitude Test (EAT‐26) (Garner 1982) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Marchiori 2012a

Rolls 2002, Rolls 2004a, Rolls 2004b, Rolls 2007a

Dieting behaviour (behaviour that involves a person restricting themselves to smaller amounts or specific types of food either to lose weight or for medical reasons) has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Van Strien 1986, Stunkard 1985

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: mood

Average (mean) mood score among study completers ‐ 7‐point agreement scale anchored (‐3) strongly disagree and (+3) strongly agree (Marchiori 2012a, Reinbach 2010) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Marchiori 2012a

Mood has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Gardner 2014

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: habitual dietary energy intake

Average (mean) dietary energy intake per diem among study completers in kcal (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Ahn 2010

Ebbeling 2007

Baseline level of dietary energy intake has the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Fyfe 2010, Birch 1991

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: habitual dietary macronutrient intake, Carbohydrate

Average (mean) carbohydrate intake as a proportion (%) of daily energy intake among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Ahn 2010

Baseline levels of macronutrient intake have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Beasley 2009, Monteleone 2003, Yeomans 2001, Rolls 1988

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: habitual dietary macronutrient intake, Protein

Average (mean) protein intake as a proportion (%) of daily energy intake among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Ahn 2010

Baseline levels of macronutrient intake have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Beasley 2009, Rolls 1988

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: habitual dietary macronutrient intake, Fat

Average (mean) fat intake as a proportion (%) of daily energy intake among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Ahn 2010

Baseline levels of macronutrient intake have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Beasley 2009, Brennan 2012, Monteleone 2003, Yeomans 2001, Rolls 1988

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: physical activity

Average (mean) daily total number of steps among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Ahn 2010

Baseline levels of physical activity have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Martins 2007

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: habitual energy expenditure

Average (mean) daily energy expenditure among study completers in kcal (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Rolls 2006a

Rolls 2006b, Rolls 2007a, Rolls 2007b (S1), Rolls 2007b (S2),

Rolls 2007b (S3), Rolls 2010a (E1), Rolls 2010b (E2)

Baseline levels of energy expenditure have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Martins 2007

N/A

Behavioural characteristics: habitual physical exercise

Average (mean) number of hours of physical exercise completed among study completers per week (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Marchiori 2012c

Baseline levels of physical exercise have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Martins 2007

N/A

Biological state: hunger

Average (mean) hunger rating among study completers – 100 mm visual analogue scale (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Biological state: hunger

Average (mean) hunger rating among study completers ‐ 3‐point rating scale (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Biological state: hunger

Average (mean) hunger rating among study completers ‐ 7‐point rating scale (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Biological state: appetitive state, Fullness

Average (mean) fullness rating among study completers – 100 mm visual analogue scale (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Shah 2011

Baseline levels of feelings of fullness (specific somatic

sensation or perceived general state of fullness (Blundell 2010)) have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Doucet 2008

N/A

Biological state: appetitive state, Satiety

Average (mean) satiety rating among study completers – 100 mm visual analogue scale (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Shah 2011

Baseline levels of feelings of satiety (specific somatic

sensation or perceived general state of being satiated (Blundell 2010)) have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Lemmens 2011

N/A

Biological state: appetitive state, Prospective consumption

Average (mean) prospective consumption rating among study completers – 100 mm visual analogue scale (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Shah 2011

Baseline levels of prospective consumption (how much participants felt they could eat now (Shah 2011)) have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption of food

Doucet 2008

N/A

Other clinical characteristics: depression

Average (mean) depression score among study completers ‐ Zung Depression Inventory (Zung 1986) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

No

Yes

Rolls 2002

Rolls 2004a, Rolls 2004b, Rolls 2007a

Baseline feelings of depression (or of affective, psychological or somatic symptoms associated with depression) have the potential to modify any effects of larger portions, packages, individual units or tableware on the selection and consumption

Grossniklaus 2010

N/A

Socioeconomic status: occupational status

Proportion (%) of study completers in employment (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Socioeconomic status: occupational status

Proportion (%) of study completers with a parent or caregiver in employment (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Socioeconomic status: education

Average (mean) number of years of education completed among study completers (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Socioeconomic status: education

Proportion (%) of study completers who completed at least some further education (greater than high school, at least some college) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Socioeconomic status: education

Proportion (%) of study completers with a parent or caregiver who completed at least some further education (greater than high school, at least some college) (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Socioeconomic status: education

Proportion (%) of study completers with a parent or caregiver who completed at least a 4‐year university degree (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Socioeconomic status: income

Proportion (%) of study completers with an individual income > USD 50,000 (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Socioeconomic status: income

Proportion (%) of study completers with a total family income > USD 50,000 (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Other measures of socioeconomic status: food insecurity

Proportion (%) of study completers living in a food insecure household (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Other measures of socioeconomic status: welfare receipt

Proportion (%) of study completers participating in the US National School Lunch Program (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Other measures of socioeconomic status: welfare receipt

Proportion (%) of study completers participating in the US School Nutrition Assistance Program (Continuous)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Overall (summary) risk of bias

Low risk, unclear risk or high risk (Categorical, nominal)

Participant characteristic

Yes

Yes

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Figuras y tablas -
Table 1. Record of conceptual model development