Scolaris Content Display Scolaris Content Display

Trifluoperacina versus fármacos antipsicóticos de primera generación de baja potencia para la esquizofrenia

Contraer todo Desplegar todo

Resumen

disponible en

Antecedentes

Los fármacos antipsicóticos son los pilares del tratamiento de la esquizofrenia. Las guías terapéuticas establecen que no hay ninguna diferencia en cuanto a la eficacia entre los compuestos antipsicóticos; sin embargo, los fármacos antipsicóticos de baja potencia suelen ser percibidos por los médicos como menos efectivos que los compuestos de alta potencia y también parecen diferir en cuanto a los efectos secundarios.

Objetivos

Revisar los efectos en la respuesta al tratamiento con trifluoperazina y antipsicóticos de baja potencia para los pacientes con esquizofrenia.

Métodos de búsqueda

Se hicieron búsquedas en el registro de ensayos del Grupo Cochrane de Esquizofrenia (Cochrane Schizophrenia Group) (noviembre 2010).

Criterios de selección

Se incluyeron todos los ensayos aleatorios que comparaban trifluoperacina con fármacos antipsicóticos de primera generación de baja potencia en pacientes con esquizofrenia o psicosis similares a la esquizofrenia.

Obtención y análisis de los datos

Los datos se extrajeron de forma independiente. Para los datos dicotómicos, se calcularon los cocientes de riesgos (CR) y sus intervalos de confianza (IC) del 95% sobre la base de un análisis de intención de tratar y el modelo de efectos aleatorios.

Resultados principales

La revisión actualmente incluye siete ensayos aleatorios con 422 participantes que compararon trifluoperazina con fármacos antipsicóticos de baja potencia. El tamaño de los estudios incluidos fue de entre 20 y 157 participantes con una duración de los estudios entre cuatro y 52 semanas. En términos generales, la generación de la secuencia, los procedimientos de asignación y el cegamiento se informaron de forma deficiente.

La trifluoperazina no fue significativamente diferente a los fármacos antipsicóticos de baja potencia en cuanto a la respuesta al tratamiento (trifluoperazina 26%, fármaco de baja potencia 27%, tres ECA, n = 120; CR 0,96; IC: 0,59 a 1,56; pruebas de calidad moderada). Tampoco hubo diferencias significativas en cuanto a la aceptabilidad del tratamiento, y el número de participantes que abandonó los estudios de forma temprana por cualquier razón fue ambiguo (trifluoperacina 20%, antipsicóticos de baja potencia 16%, 3 ECA, n = 239; CR 1,25; IC: 0,72 a 2,17; pruebas de baja de calidad). No hubo diferencias significativas en el número de pacientes con al menos un efecto adverso (trifluoperazina 60%, fármaco de baja potencia 38%, un ECA, n = 60; CR 1,60; IC: 0,94 a 2,74; pruebas de calidad moderada). Sin embargo, al menos un trastorno del movimiento fue significativamente más frecuente en el grupo de trifluoperazina (trifluoperazina 23%, antipsicóticos de baja potencia 13%, dos ECA, n = 123; CR 2,08; IC: 0,78 a 5,55; pruebas de muy baja calidad) así como la incoordinación (trifluoperazina 20%, fármaco de baja potencia 5%, un ECA, n = 60; CR 7,00; IC 1,60 a 30,66) y la rigidez (trifluoperazina 45%, fármaco de baja potencia 10%, un ECA, n = 60; CR 4,50; IC: 1,58 a 12,84). No hubo datos disponibles para otros resultados de interés, la muerte, la sedación y la calidad de vida.

Conclusiones de los autores

Los resultados no mostraron una diferencia en cuanto a la eficacia entre la trifluoperazina y los antipsicóticos de baja potencia. La trifluoperazina produjo más trastornos del movimiento. El número de estudios aleatorios es bajo, al igual que su calidad, la calidad de las pruebas para los resultados de interés varió de moderada a muy baja, de manera que se necesitarían estudios adicionales más nuevos para establecer conclusiones acerca de los efectos relativos de la trifluoperazina y los antipsicóticos de baja potencia.

PICO

Population
Intervention
Comparison
Outcome

El uso y la enseñanza del modelo PICO están muy extendidos en el ámbito de la atención sanitaria basada en la evidencia para formular preguntas y estrategias de búsqueda y para caracterizar estudios o metanálisis clínicos. PICO son las siglas en inglés de cuatro posibles componentes de una pregunta de investigación: paciente, población o problema; intervención; comparación; desenlace (outcome).

Para saber más sobre el uso del modelo PICO, puede consultar el Manual Cochrane.

Resumen en términos sencillos

Trifluoperacina versus fármacos antipsicóticos de primera generación de baja potencia para la esquizofrenia

Los antipsicóticos son el tratamiento principal para la esquizofrenia y ayudan a los pacientes a afrontar los síntomas positivos que incluyen oír voces, ver cosas y tener creencias extrañas. Las guías establecen que no hay ninguna diferencia en la efectividad entre los antipsicóticos, aunque los fármacos antipsicóticos de baja potencia suelen ser considerados como menos efectivos que los fármacos de alta potencia, y también parecen diferir en cuanto a los efectos secundarios. La clasificación en medicación de alta potencia y de baja potencia significa que se necesitan dosis mayores de los fármacos antipsicóticos de baja potencia para tratar los síntomas de la esquizofrenia. Los efectos secundarios frecuentes de la mayoría de los fármacos antipsicóticos de alta potencia incluyen trastornos del movimiento como movimientos incontrolables de la cara, los brazos o las piernas; temblores; problemas con el equilibrio o la marcha; inquietud; convulsiones; dolor en las articulaciones, mientras que es más probable que los fármacos de baja potencia causen sedación, fiebre y pérdida de la fuerza muscular. La investigación no ha evaluado ni comparado los fármacos antipsicóticos de alta potencia con los de baja potencia. Por lo tanto, el objetivo de la revisión fue comparar la trifluoperazina (un antipsicótico de alta potencia) con antipsicóticos de baja potencia para los pacientes con esquizofrenia. Los ejemplos típicos de fármacos de baja potencia son clorpromazina, clorprotixeno, tioridazina o levomepromazina. La revisión se basa en una búsqueda efectuada en el año 2010 e incluyó siete estudios con un total de 422 personas.Comparó trifluoperazina con fármacos antipsicóticos de baja potencia. En general, la información se describió de manera deficiente y la calidad de los estudios fue baja; los autores consideraron que la calidad de las pruebas para los resultados principales de interés fue moderada, baja o muy baja. Los resultados no muestran una superioridad de la trifluoperazina comparada con los antipsicóticos de baja potencia. Sin embargo, al menos un trastorno del movimiento (rigidez muscular) fue significativamente mayor con trifluoperazina. Para los pacientes con esquizofrenia, es importante saber que la trifluoperazina y los antipsicóticos de baja potencia son aproximadamente iguales para tratar síntomas como oír voces o ver cosas. Difieren levemente en sus efectos secundarios, y la trifluoperazina da lugar a al menos un trastorno del movimiento (rigidez muscular). Sin embargo, no se encontró una superioridad clara de la trifluoperazina versus los antipsicóticos de baja potencia. Debido al limitado número de estudios, participantes y la baja calidad de la información, estos resultados deben ser interpretados con precaución.

Este resumen en términos sencillos ha sido redactado por un consumidor, Benjamin Gray, Usuario de Servicios y Experto Usuario de Servicios, Rethink Mental Illness.

Authors' conclusions

Implications for practice

1. For clinicians

Clinicians should know that we did not find differences in the efficacy between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics, but that low‐potency antipsychotics appear to produce fewer movement disorders (measured as 'at least one movement disorder' and rigor). Due to the limited number of studies and participants, and due to low quality, the results have to be interpreted with caution.

2. For people with schizophrenia

For people with schizophrenia it is important to know that there is low quality evidence that trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics are approximately equal in their effects on treatment response, and that there is evidence of lower quality that they clearly differ in side‐effects (such as movement disorders).

They might tell their doctors that they want to be involved in the choice of the antipsychotic that is best for them.

3. For managers/policy makers

There were no data on rehospitalisation, economic outcomes, healthy days or quality of life, which can be considered very important outcomes for decision makers. Thus, it is not possible to make any recommendations apart from the fact that all of the examined drugs in this review have lost their patent protection and are therefore rather inexpensive.

Implications for research

1. General

The outcome reporting about the effects of trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotics on clinical response was insufficient. Strict adherence to the CONSORT statement (Moher 2010) would make such studies much more informative.

2. Specific
2.1 Reviews

Studies we have had to exclude because they were not directly relevant, however, do still show how this compound has been evaluated in many other ways. Some of these remain clinically relevant and may merit further systematic reviews (Table 2).

Open in table viewer
Table 2. Other reviews suggested by excluded studies

Suggested title

Excluded study tag

Alternate use of different antipsychotics for schizophrenia

Wilson 1961 (trifluoperazine and chlorpromazine)

Combined multi‐drug therapy for schizophrenia

Casey 1961

Trifluoperazine and chlorpromazine combination for schizophrenia

DeWolfe 1971, Smith 1958

Trifluoperazine doses for schizophrenia

Crane 1970, Prien 1969

Trifluoperazine versus mid‐potency antipsychotics for schizophrenia

Gardos 1970 (thiothixene), Hamilton 1963 (prochlorperazine)

Trifluoperazine versus other high‐potency antipsychotics and anxiolytics for schizophrenia

Marjerrison 1966 (triperidol, haloanisone), Prien 1968

Trifluoperazine versus other phenothiazine (unknown) antipsychotics for schizophrenia

Rudy 1958

Trifluoperazine versus placebo

Hershon 1972, Platz 1967, Prien 1969, Weston 1961

2.2 Trials

Even the number of studies providing data on the primary outcome response to treatment, let alone on most adverse events was very low, as well as the overall quality of the included studies. Thus it would be warranted to have newer and longer studies studying the difference between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotic drugs, as first‐generation antipsychotic drugs are still frequently prescribed, not only in poorer countries but also in countries for example, Germany. We suggest a design of a future study in Table 3.

Open in table viewer
Table 3. Design of a future study

Methods

Allocation: randomised ‐ clearly described generation of sequence and concealment of allocation.
Blinding: double ‐ described and tested.
Duration: long term.

Participants

People with schizophrenia or schizophrenia‐like disorder.
N = 500.
Age: any.
Sex: both.
History: any.

Interventions

1. Trifluoperazine (oral).

2. Any low‐potency antipsychotic (oral).

Outcomes

Response (primary outcome)

Rehospitalisation

Mental state (BPRS)

Global state (CGI)

Leaving the study early (including specific causes)

Death (natural and unnatural causes)

Side‐effects

Quality of life

Satisfaction with care

Employment

BPRS ‐ Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale
CGI ‐ Clinical Global Impression

Summary of findings

Open in table viewer
Summary of findings for the main comparison. Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia

Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia

Patient or population: patients with schizophrenia
Settings: Inpatients and outpatients
Intervention: Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of Participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Control

Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Response to treatment ‐ medium term
Follow‐up: 4‐6 months

Study population

RR 0.96
(0.59 to 1.56)

120
(3 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
low1,2

364 per 1000

349 per 1000
(215 to 567)

Moderate

350 per 1000

336 per 1000
(206 to 546)

Leaving the study early: 1. Acceptability of treatment ‐ leaving early due to any reason
Follow‐up: 1‐4 months

Study population

RR 1.25
(0.72 to 2.17)

239
(3 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
low1,2

160 per 1000

200 per 1000
(115 to 348)

Moderate

182 per 1000

228 per 1000
(131 to 395)

Adverse events: 1. General ‐ at least one adverse effect
Follow‐up: mean 4 months

Study population

RR 1.6
(0.94 to 2.74)

60
(1 study)

⊕⊕⊕⊝
moderate1

375 per 1000

600 per 1000
(352 to 1000)

Moderate

375 per 1000

600 per 1000
(352 to 1000)

Adverse events: 2.a. Specific ‐ movement disorders ‐ at least one movement disorder
Follow‐up: 6‐26 weeks

Study population

RR 2.08
(0.78 to 5.55)

123
(2 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
low1,3

89 per 1000

186 per 1000
(70 to 496)

Moderate

50 per 1000

104 per 1000
(39 to 278)

Death

See comment

See comment

Not estimable

0
(04)

See comment

Sedation

See comment

See comment

Not estimable

0
(05)

See comment

Quality of life

See comment

See comment

Not estimable

0
(05)

See comment

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1 Risk of bias: rated 'serious' ‐ most information is from studies at unclear risk of bias, missing or unclear results for incomplete outcome data and selective reporting
2 Imprecision: rated 'serious' ‐ the total number of events is less than 300 and the estimate of effect includes appreciable benefit/harm.
3 Imprecision: rated 'serious' ‐ the total number of events is less than 300 and the CI is quite wide
4 There were no data for this very important outcome
5 There were no data for this outcome

Background

Description of the condition

Schizophrenia is often a chronic and disabling psychiatric disorder. It afflicts approximately one per cent of the population world‐wide with little gender differences. Its typical manifestations are 'positive' symptoms such as fixed, false beliefs (delusions) and perceptions without cause (hallucinations), 'negative' symptoms such as apathy and lack of drive, disorganisation of behaviour and thought, and catatonic symptoms such as mannerisms and bizarre posturing (Carpenter 1994). The degree of suffering and disability is considerable with 80% to 90% of people with schizophrenia not working (Marvaha 2004) and up to 10% dying by suicide (Tsuang 1978).

Description of the intervention

Antipsychotic drugs are the mainstay of treatment for schizophrenia. They can be classified according to their biochemical structure (e.g. butyrophenones, phenothiazines, thioxanthenes etc), their risk of producing movement disorders ('atypical' versus 'typical' antipsychotics) and the doses necessary for an antipsychotic effect (high‐potency versus low‐potency antipsychotics). The classification into high‐potency and low‐potency medication means that for low‐potency antipsychotic drugs higher doses are necessary to obtain the same dopamine receptor occupancy and efficacy (Seeman 1975) than for high‐potency antipsychotic drugs. In this context, trifluoperazine belongs to the high‐potency antipsychotic drug group. It is a conventional antipsychotic drug with a primary indication for schizophrenia.

Typical examples for low‐potency drugs are chlorpromazine, chlorprothixene, thioridazine or levomepromazine. It is an old psychiatric dogma that can be found in textbooks and guidelines that ‐ with the exception of clozapine ‐ there is no difference in efficacy between any antipsychotic compounds (Gaebel 2006; Lehman 2004). Nevertheless, low‐potency antipsychotic drugs are often clinically perceived as less efficacious than high‐potency compounds, and high‐ and low‐potency antipsychotics also seem to differ in their side‐effects. For example, low‐potency drugs are more likely to cause sedation or hypotonia, whereas high‐potency drugs are more prone to produce extrapyramidal side‐effects. This is one of a series of similar reviews on the question whether there are differences between high‐potency and low‐potency antipsychotics (Table 1).

Open in table viewer
Table 1. Series of similar reviews

Title

Reference

Haloperidol versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Tardy 2011b

Flupenthixol versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Tardy 2011

Fluphenazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Tardy 2011a

Perphenazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Tardy 2011c

How the intervention might work

Schizophrenia is considered to be a chronic disorder caused by hyper‐dopaminergic states in the limbic system (Berger 2003). All antipsychotic drugs are believed to exert their effects by blocking, to a greater or lesser extent, D2‐receptors in the brain.

Trifluoperazine is a phenothiazine that is considered a high‐potency antipsychotic along with drugs such as haloperidol and fluphenazine. In higher doses it is used in the treatment of psychiatric illnesses. In lower doses it has also been used in the management of nausea and vomiting. It works by blocking dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in the mesocortical and mesolimbic pathways, thereby minimising symptoms of schizophrenia, such as hallucinations and delusions. Possible side‐effects, common to high‐potency antipsychotics, include uncontrollable movements of face, arms, or legs, tremor, problems with balance or walking, restlessness, seizures, joint pain or unusual thoughts or behaviour.

Low‐potency medications have a lower affinity for dopamine receptors so that a higher dose is required to effectively treat symptoms of schizophrenia. In addition, they block other than dopamine receptors, such as cholinergic or histaminergic receptors. This also explains the occurrence of their side‐effects, mostly sedation, hypotonia or fever, which occur less frequently with high‐potency drugs. The distinction between high‐ and low‐potency drugs is not entirely clear, but attempts have been made to express this in terms of dose equivalence. The most frequently applied concept is based on chlorpromazine equivalents. Davis 1974 and Haase 1983, among others, provide data about comparable doses of various antipsychotic drugs to achieve an effect similar to 100 mg chlorpromazine.

Why it is important to do this review

Cochrane systematic reviews on the effects of specific conventional antipsychotic drugs have compared the effects of one antipsychotic drug versus any other antipsychotic drug (e.g. Pimozide versus any other antipsychotic drug (Rathbone 2007). Systematic reviews have not evaluated the comparative effects of high‐potency and low‐potency antipsychotic drugs. As a result, many guidelines, such as the German national schizophrenia guidelines (Gaebel 2006), or that of the World Federations of Societies of Biological Psychiatry (Falkai 2005), state that “all conventional antipsychotics if adequately dosed have comparable efficacy.” These guidelines contrast with a clinical impression that low‐potency conventional antipsychotic drugs are less efficacious than high‐potency conventional antipsychotic drugs and that their side‐effects differ considerably.

Conventional antipsychotic drugs are still the mainstay of treatment in countries that can not afford newer, expensive "atypical" or "second‐generation" antipsychotic drugs. But even in some industrialised countries such as Germany, conventional antipsychotic medications still account for 50% of the market‐share (Lohse 2005). Recent studies have also called into question the superiority of the more expensive second‐generation antipsychotics (Leucht 2009; Lieberman 2005; Jones 2006). Therefore, research on conventional antipsychotic agents is essential. The results of this review could aid our understanding of the comparative efficacy and safety of conventional antipsychotic drugs.

Objectives

To evaluate the effects of the high‐potency antipsychotic drug trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs.

Methods

Criteria for considering studies for this review

Types of studies

We included all randomised controlled (parallel group or cross‐over) trials irrespective of duration. We included trials that were described as randomised or where randomisation was implied. We excluded quasi‐randomised trials, such as those that used alternation, an open list of random numbers, or any other method of recruitment where allocation to interventions was predictable.

Types of participants

People with schizophrenia and schizophrenia‐like psychoses (schizophreniform and schizoaffective disorders). There is no clear evidence that the schizophrenia‐like psychoses are caused by fundamentally different disease processes or require different treatment approaches (Carpenter 1994). We included studies irrespective of the diagnostic criteria used. Diagnostic criteria, such as ICD 10 (International Classification of Diseases, tenth revision) or DSM‐IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental disorders, version IV), are not meticulously used in clinical practice and restricting inclusion to trials that used operationally defined diagnostic criteria would have reduced generalisation and representativeness.

We were interested in making sure that information is as relevant to the current care of people with schizophrenia as possible so proposed to clearly highlight the current clinical state (acute, early post‐acute, partial remission, remission) as well as the stage (prodromal, first episode, early illness, persistent) and as to whether the studies primarily focused on people with particular problems (for example, negative symptoms, treatment‐resistant illnesses).

Types of interventions

1. Intervention: Trifluoperazine

Any dose of oral mode of administration (no depots, no short‐acting parenteral forms of administration).

2. Comparators: Low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Low‐potency conventional antipsychotic drugs in any oral form of administration and used at any dose. We used the dose equivalence tables by Davis 1974 and/or Haase 1983 to define drugs as low‐potency if the chlorpromazine equivalence was roughly equal or higher than chlorpromazine. The chlorpromazine equivalences of sulpiride are often estimated to be approximately 100. However, its properties are similar to those of amisulpride, which is an atypical antipsychotic and not within the scope of this review. Moreover, sulpiride does not cause a lot of sedation, which is another important characteristic of low‐potency antipsychotics. Therefore, we decided that we would not consider sulpiride in this review.

Types of outcome measures

Primary outcomes
1. Clinical response

Response to treatment as defined by the original studies

Secondary outcomes
1. Mental state: symptoms of schizophrenia

1.1 Overall symptoms ‐ average score/change in general mental state
1.2 Positive symptoms ‐ average score/change in positive symptoms
1.3 Negative symptoms ‐ average score/change in negative symptoms

2. Global state: average score/change in global state
3. Leaving the study early

3.1 Acceptability of treatment ‐ leaving the study early due to any reason
3.2 Leaving the study early due to inefficacy of treatment
3.3 Leaving the study early due to side‐effects

4. Relapse ‐ as defined by each of the studies
5. Service use

5.1 Rehospitalisation

6. Adverse effects

6.1 At least one adverse effect
6.2 Extrapyramidal/movement disorders
6.2.1 At least one movement disorder
6.2.2. Various movement disorders
6.3 Cardiac effects
6.4 Hypotension
6.5 Sedation
6.6 Weight gain
6.7 Other

7. Death

7.1 Death (all causes)
7.2 Suicide

8. Quality of life
9. Participant's/carer`s satisfaction with care
10. Economic outcomes

Outcomes were classified into short term (up to 12 weeks), medium term (up to 26 weeks) and long term (longer than 26 weeks).

Search methods for identification of studies

No language restriction was applied

Electronic searches

Cochrane Schizophrenia Group's Trials Register (November 2010)

We searched the register using the phrase:

[(*trifluoperazine* in intervention of STUDY) OR (*trifluoperazine* in title, abstract and index terms of REFERENCE entered >= 01/05/10)]

This register is compiled by systematic searches of major databases, clinical trial registries, handsearches and conference proceedings (see Group Module).

Searching other resources

1. Reference searching

We inspected the references of all identified included studies for more trials.

2. Previous reviews

We searched previous conventional reviews (e.g. Davis 1989; Klein 1969).

3. Personal contact

We contacted the first author of each included study for missing information and for the existence of further studies.

4. Drug companies

We contacted the original manufacturer of trifluoperazine and asked for further relevant studies and for missing information on identified studies.

Data collection and analysis

Selection of studies

Review authors (MT, MD) independently inspected all abstracts identified in the searches. Disagreement was resolved by discussion and where doubt still remained, we acquired the full article for further inspection. Once the full articles were obtained, at least two review authors independently decided whether the studies met the review criteria. If disagreement could not be resolved by discussion, we resolved it with a third review author (SL) or sought further information from the study authors.

Data extraction and management

1. Extraction

Originally two review authors (MT, MD) independently extracted data from all selected trials. We decided post‐hoc to include all outcomes reported by a study, not only the predefined outcomes in the methods section. For the outcomes added post‐hoc only a random sample of 25% were independently extracted by a second review authors (MH, see Acknowledgements). When disagreement arose, we resolved it by discussion with a third review author (SL). Where this was not possible, we contacted the study authors to resolve the dilemma.

2. Management
2.1 Forms

We extracted data onto simple standard forms.

2.2 Scale‐derived data

If reported, we planned to include continuous data from rating scales only if: (a) the psychometric properties of the measuring instrument had been described in a peer‐reviewed journal (Marshall 2000); (b) the measuring instrument was not written or modified by one of the trialists.

2.3 Endpoint versus change data

Since there is no principal statistical reason why endpoint and change data should measure different effects (Higgins 2011), we decided primarily to use scale endpoint data. If endpoint data were not available we used change data.

2.4 Skewed data

Continuous data on clinical and social outcomes are often not normally distributed. To avoid the pitfall of applying parametric tests to non‐parametric data, we applied the following standards to all data before inclusion: (a) data from studies of at least 200 participants were entered in the analysis irrespective of the following rules, because skewed data pose less of a problem in large studies; (b) endpoint data: when a scale starts from the finite number zero, we subtracted the lowest possible value from the mean, and divided this by the standard deviation (SD). If this value was lower than one, it strongly suggested a skew and the study was excluded. If this ratio was higher than one but below two, there is suggestion of skew. We entered the study and tested whether its inclusion or exclusion substantially changed the results. If the ratio was larger than two the study was included, because skew is less likely (Altman 1996; Higgins 2008); (c) change data: when continuous data are presented on a scale which includes a possibility of negative values (such as change data), it is difficult to tell whether data are skewed or not. We entered the study, because change data tend to be less skewed and because excluding studies would also lead to bias, since not all the available information was used.

2.5 Common measure

To facilitate comparison between trials, we intended to convert variables that can be reported in different metrics, such as days in hospital (mean days per year, per week or per month) to a common metric (e.g. mean days per month).

2.6 'Summary of findings' table

We used the GRADE approach to interpret findings (Schünemann 2008) and used GRADE profiler (GRADE) to import data from RevMan 5 (Review Manager) to create summary of findings Table for the main comparison. This table provides outcome‐specific information concerning the overall quality of evidence from each included study in the comparison, the magnitude of effect of the interventions examined, and the sum of available data on all outcomes we rated as important to patient‐care and decision making. We selected the following long‐term main outcomes for inclusion in the 'Summary of findings' table.

  1. Response to treatment

  2. Acceptability of treatment ‐ leaving the study early due to any reason

  3. Adverse effects ‐ at least one adverse effect

  4. Adverse effects ‐ at least one extrapyramidal side‐effect

  5. Adverse effects ‐ sedation

  6. Death

  7. Quality of life

Assessment of risk of bias in included studies

Review authors MT, MD worked independently by using criteria described in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011) to assess trial quality. This new set of criteria is based on evidence of associations between overestimate of effect and high risk of bias due to mistakes in sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding, incomplete outcome data and selective reporting.

Disagreements, if any, were resolved by referring to the trial report, correspondence with the authors of the report and through discussions.and involvement of another member of the review group (SL). We reported any quality assessments where there was lack of agreement between raters.

Where inadequate details of randomisation and other characteristics of trials were provided, we contacted the authors of the studies in order to obtain additional information.

We have noted the level of risk of bias in both the text of the review and in the summary of findings Table for the main comparison.

Measures of treatment effect

1. Dichotomous data

For binary outcomes we calculated a standard estimation of the random‐effects (Der‐Simonian 1986) risk ratio (RR) and its 95% confidence interval (CI). It has been shown that RR is more intuitive (Boissel 1999) than odds ratios and that odds ratios tend to be interpreted as RR by clinicians (Deeks 2000). This misinterpretation then leads to an overestimate of the impression of the effect. Where possible, efforts were made to convert outcome measures to dichotomous data. This could be done by identifying cut‐off points on rating scales and dividing participants accordingly into 'clinically improved' or 'not clinically improved'. It was generally assumed that if there had been a 50% reduction in a scale‐derived score such as the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS, Overall 1962) or the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS, Kay 1986), this could be considered as a response to treatment (Leucht 2005a; Leucht 2005b). If data based on these thresholds were not available, we used the primary cut‐off presented by the original authors.

2. Continuous data

If continuous data been reported, we would have estimated a mean difference (MD) between groups. If scales were of such similarity to allow pooling we would have calculated the standardised mean difference (SMD) and, whenever possible, transformed the effect back to the units of one or more of the specific instruments.

Unit of analysis issues

1. Cluster trials

Studies increasingly employ 'cluster randomisation' (such as randomisation by clinician or practice) but analysis and pooling of clustered data poses problems. Firstly, authors often fail to account for intraclass correlation in clustered studies, leading to a 'unit of analysis' error (Divine 1992) whereby P values are spuriously low, confidence intervals unduly narrow and statistical significance overestimated. This causes type I errors (Bland 1997; Gulliford 1999).

If we had included cluster randomised trials and if results from trials had not adjusted for clustering, we would have attempted to adjust the results for clustering, by multiplying the standard errors of the effect estimates (risk ratio or mean difference, ignoring clustering) by the square root of the design effect. The design effect is calculated as DEff =1 + (M ‐ 1) ICC, where M is the average cluster size and ICC is the intra‐cluster coefficient (Higgins 2008). If an ICC was not available from the trial, other sources would have been used to impute ICCs (Campbell 2000).

If clustering had been incorporated into the analysis of primary studies, we would have presented these data as if from a non‐cluster randomised study, but adjusted for the clustering effect. If a cluster study had been appropriately analysed taking into account ICC and relevant data documented in the report, synthesis with parallel group randomised trials would have been possible using the generic inverse variance technique, where the natural logarithm of the effect estimate (and standard errors) for all included trials for that outcome would be calculated and entered into RevMan along with the log of the effect estimate (and standard errors) from the cluster randomised trial(s). We would have used methods described in section 7.7.7.2 and 7.7.7.3 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2008) to obtain standard errors.

2. Cross‐over trials

A major concern of cross‐over trials is the carry‐over effect. It occurs if an effect (e.g. pharmacological, physiological or psychological) of the treatment in the first phase is carried over to the second phase. As a consequence, on entry to the second phase the participants can differ systematically from their initial state despite a wash‐out phase. For the same reason cross‐over trials are not appropriate if the condition of interest is unstable (Elbourne 2002). As both effects are very likely in schizophrenia, randomised cross‐over studies were eligible but only data up to the point of first cross‐over were used for analysis.

Dealing with missing data

1. Overall loss of credibility

At some degree of loss of follow‐up, data must lose credibility (Xia 2009). The loss to follow‐up in randomised schizophrenia trials is often considerable calling the validity of the results into question. Nevertheless, it is unclear which degree of attrition leads to a high degree of bias. We did not exclude trials from outcomes on the basis of the percentage of participants completing them. We, however, used the 'Risk of bias' tool described above to indicate potential bias when more than 25% of the participants left the studies prematurely, when the reasons for attrition differed between the intervention and the control group, and when no appropriate imputation strategies were applied.

2. Dichotomous data

Data were presented on a 'once‐randomised‐always‐analyse' basis, assuming an intention‐to‐treat analysis. If the authors applied such a strategy, we used their results. If the original authors presented only the results of the per‐protocol or completer population, we assumed that those participants lost to follow‐up would have had the same percentage of events as those who remained in the study.

3. Continuous data
3.1 General

Intention‐to‐treat (ITT) was used when available. We anticipated that in some studies, in order to do an ITT analysis, the method of last observation carried forward (LOCF) would be employed within the study report. As with all methods of imputation to deal with missing data, LOCF introduces uncertainty about the reliability of the results (Leon 2006). Therefore, if LOCF data had been used in the analysis, we would have indicated this in the review.

3.2 Missing standard deviations

Where there are missing measures of variance for continuous data but an exact standard error and confidence interval are available for group means, and either ‘P’ value or 't' value are available for differences in mean, we can calculate standard deviation values according to method described in Section 7.7.3 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011). If standard deviations were not reported and could not be calculated from available data, we asked authors to supply the data. In the absence of data from authors, we used the mean standard deviation from other studies.

Assessment of heterogeneity

1. Clinical

We considered all included studies without any comparison to judge clinical heterogeneity.

We inspected all studies for clearly outlying situations or people which we had not predicted would arise and discussed them fully, if such situations or participants arose.

2. Methodological

We considered all included studies initially, without seeing comparison data, to judge methodological heterogeneity. We inspected all studies for clearly outlying methods which we had not predicted would arise and discussed them if they were evident.

3. Statistical
3.1 Visual inspection

We visually inspected graphs to identify trials with non‐overlapping confidence intervals within a forest plot to suggest the possibility of statistical heterogeneity.

3.2 Employing the I2statistic

Heterogeneity between studies was investigated by considering the I2 method alongside the Chi2 'P' value. The I2 provides an estimate of the percentage of inconsistency thought to be due to chance (Higgins 2011). The importance of the observed value of I2 depends on i. magnitude and direction of effects and ii. strength of evidence for heterogeneity (e.g. 'P' value from Chi2 test, or a confidence interval for I2).

An I2 estimate greater than or equal to 50% accompanied by a statistically significant Chi2 statistic, was interpreted as evidence of substantial levels of heterogeneity (Section 9.5.2 Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of InterventionsHiggins 2011) and reasons for heterogeneity were explored. If the inconsistency was high and the clear reasons were found, data were presented separately.

Assessment of reporting biases

We intended to enter data from all identified and selected trials for each outcome into a funnel plot (trial effect versus trial size) in an attempt to investigate the likelihood of overt publication bias. We planned to test for funnel plot asymmetry only for outcomes where there were 10 or more studies and if the studies were not of similar sizes, as recommended in Section 10.4.3.1 of the Cochrane Handbook (Higgins 2011). If outcomes with more than 10 studies had been available, we would have used the statistical test by Egger (Egger 1997) to formally assess funnel‐plot asymmetry, and supplement visual inspection of the forest plot to differentiate small‐study effects from other reasons for funnel plot asymmetry.

Data synthesis

We employed a random‐effects model for analyses (Der‐Simonian 1986). We understand that there is no closed argument for preference for use of fixed‐effect or random‐effects models. The random‐effects method incorporates an assumption that the different studies are estimating different, yet related, intervention effects. This does seem true to us and the random‐effects model takes into account differences between studies, even if there is no statistically significant heterogeneity. Therefore, the random‐effects model is usually more conservative in terms of statistical significance, although as a disadvantage it puts added weight onto smaller studies which can either inflate or deflate the effect size. We examined in a sensitivity analysis whether using a fixed‐effect model markedly changed the results of the primary outcome.

Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity

1. Subgroup analysis

Subgroup analyses were applied only to the primary outcome.

1.1 Comparison drug

In order to reduce clinical and methodological reasons for heterogeneity, we subgrouped trials that compared trifluoperazine versus each single low‐potency antipsychotic separately.

1.2 Clinical state, stage or problem

We proposed to undertake this review and provide an overview of the effects of trifluoperazine for people with schizophrenia in general. In addition, however, we tried to report data on subgroups of people in the same clinical state, stage and with similar problems but data were not available.

2. Investigation of heterogeneity

When statistical heterogeneity was detected and quantified as significant, we noted whether significant heterogeneity was present within the subgroups. If unanticipated clinical or methodological heterogeneity was evident among the trials in the subgroups, or in the pooled results, we stated hypotheses regarding these for future reviews or versions of this review. We did not undertake further analyses relating to these hypotheses.

Sensitivity analysis

Sensitivity analyses were only applied to the primary outcome.

1. Risk of bias

We analysed the effects of excluding trials that were judged to be at high risk of bias across one or more of the domains of randomisation (implied as randomised with no further details available) allocation concealment, blinding and outcome reporting for the meta‐analysis of the primary outcome. If the exclusion of trials at high risk of bias did not substantially alter the direction of effect or the precision of the effect estimates, then data from these trials were included in the analysis.

2. Assessment of dosage

We included trials in a sensitivity analysis if doses between high‐potency and low‐potency antipsychotics were clearly discrepant by our judgement based on the chlorpromazine equivalence tables by Davis 1974, Haase 1983 and Andreasen 2010. If there was no substantive difference when studies with discrepant doses were added, then all data were employed.

3. Imputed values

If we had included cluster randomised trials, we planned to undertake a sensitivity analysis to assess the effects of including data from trials where we used imputed values for ICC in calculating the design effect in cluster randomised trials.

If substantial differences were noted in the direction or precision of effect estimates in any of the sensitivity analyses listed above, we did not pool data from the excluded trials with the other trials contributing to the outcome, but presented them separately

4. Fixed and random effects

We synthesised data for the primary outcome using a fixed‐effect model to evaluate whether the greater weights assigned to larger trials with greater event rates altered the significance of the results, compared to the more evenly distributed weights in the random‐effects model.

Results

Description of studies

For substantive description of studies please see Characteristics of included studies and Characteristics of excluded studies tables.

Results of the search

The search in the Cochrane Schizophrenia Group register yielded 216 references from which 181 studies were identified and screened. Thirty‐four studies were closely inspected. Eight publications on seven studies were included and 27 publications reporting on 27 studies were excluded (see Figure 1).


Study flow diagram.

Study flow diagram.

Included studies

Seven studies (422 participants) met the inclusion criteria.

1. Length of trials

Of the included studies, one study had a duration up to one month and two a duration up to three months. Two studies had a duration up to four months, one study lasted up to six months and one study up to 12 months.

2. Participants

In five included studies, participants were diagnosed according to clinical diagnoses. One study diagnosed according to the Present State Examination (Leff 1971), one study diagnosed participants according to Bleuler´s concept (Reardon 1966).

3. Setting

Four studies were conducted in hospitals and one study included outpatients. Two studies (Coons 1962; Hollister 1960) did not report on setting.

4. Study size

Hanlon 1965 was the largest study with 157 participants, followed by Coons 1962 with 103 participants. Leff 1971 and Morton 1968 were the smallest studies, each randomising only 20 participants with schizophrenia. Three further studies randomised between 30 and 80 participants.

5. Interventions

All studies compared trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics. In most studies flexible doses of antipsychotic drugs could be applied. The dose ranges were: 100‐1800 mg/day for chlorpromazine, 100 to 1000 mg/day for thioridazine and 2 to 50 mg/day for trifluoperazine. In two studies various low‐potency antipsychotics could be administered (Hanlon 1965; Schiele 1961).

6. Outcomes
6.1 Response to treatment

Our primary outcome was response to treatment as defined by the original studies. Different definitions were used. In one study (Hollister 1960), the ward psychiatrist decided whether patients had responded or not. Morton 1968 applied an overall clinical assessment with the Wing scale (Wing 1961, see below) and combined this with the raters' general clinical impressions. Schiele 1961 used the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (Hathaway 1940, see below).

6.2 Relapse

Five of the included studies reported data on relapse (Hollister 1960; Leff 1971; Morton 1968; Reardon 1966; Schiele 1961).

6.3 Leaving the study early

The number of participants leaving the study early were recorded for the categories any reason, adverse events and lack of efficacy. Three out of seven included studies reported on this outcome (Hanlon 1965; Reardon 1966; Schiele 1961).

6.4 Service use

None of the included studies reported data on this outcome.

6.5 Adverse effects

The following adverse effects 'at least one adverse event', at least one movement disorder, akathisia, akinesia, dyskinesia, incoordination, rigor, atypical seizure, blurred vision, decreased appetite, dizziness, drooling, drowsiness, insomnia, lethargy, muscle weakness, polydipsia, polyuria, skin disorder, syncope, slurred speech and tenseness were reported in a dichotomous manner in terms of the number of participants with a given side‐effect.

6.6 Missing outcomes

None of the included studies reported data on death, sedation, weight gain, quality of life, participant´s satisfaction with care and economic outcomes.

6.7 Scale data

The presentation of scale derived data was very poor. Mean values could not be used. The following scales were used by the original authors to classify participants as responders and non‐responders.

6.7.1 Wing scale (Wing 1961)

A symptom rating scale, measuring affect, speech and delusions. High scores mean more symptoms.

6.7.2 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (Hathaway 1940)

A personality test in mental health. Schiele 1961 used it for the classification into responders and non‐responders, but the exact procedure remained unclear.

Excluded studies

Twenty‐seven studies were excluded from the analysis. Six studies were excluded because of inadequate allocation, they were not randomised (Barsa 1959; Childers 1964; Freyhan 1959; Gillis 1977; Terminska 1989; Vinar 1968).

Altogether 14 studies were excluded because of inappropriate interventions for this review. Of these, three compared trifluoperazine against placebo (Crane 1970; Hershon 1972; Weston 1961), five examined combination treatment (Casey 1961; DeWolfe 1971; Prien 1969; Smith 1958; Wilson 1961), Hamilton 1963 and Gardos 1970 compared trifluoperazine with mid‐potency antipsychotics, two studies compared trifluoperazine with another high‐potency antipsychotic (triperidol) (Marjerrison 1966; Prien 1968) and one study compared trifluoperazine with other phenothiazine derivatives, but did not report exactly which ones (Rudy 1958). One study did not include trifluoperazine (Welbel 1980). Seven studies were excluded because they did not present any usable data for this review (de Jong 1965; Eitan 1992; Jones 1971; Platz 1967; Spiegel 1967; Teja 1975; Vestre 1970).

Risk of bias in included studies

For graphical representations of our judgements of risk of bias please refer to Figure 2 and Figure 3. Full details of judgements are presented in the 'Risk of bias' tables.


'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.


'Risk of bias' summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

'Risk of bias' summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

Allocation

All studies were judged with an unclear risk of bias concerning random sequence generation, because all were described as randomised but none gave further information on sequence generation.

Two studies were rated with low risk of bias on allocation concealment; both reported that the only ones with access to the identity of patients were the unit secretary (Leff 1971) or the hospital pharmacist (Morton 1968). Five of the included randomised studies did not provide details on allocation concealment and were thus rated as 'unclear'.

Blinding

Four studies were double‐blind and provided at least some details about the process (usually identical capsules) so that they were rated as low risk of bias in terms of blinding (Coons 1962; Hollister 1960; Leff 1971; Schiele 1961). In Hanlon 1965 there could have been a performance bias, because treating ward physicians were aware of the various drugs and dosages involved in the study. Morton 1968 was a double‐blind study, but the nurses correctly guessed the drugs participants were assigned to. Thus blinding did not work in this study. Reardon 1966 just stated that “neither the ward personnel nor the investigators knew which drug the patients received”, but there was no detail about the blinding methods. We classified the study as 'unclear' risk in terms of blinding.

Incomplete outcome data

One study was judged to have a low risk of bias (Schiele 1961). Four studies were judged unclear in terms of incomplete outcome data. Of these, Hollister 1960 and Morton 1968 did not address whether there were drop‐outs. Leff 1971 did not report drop‐outs separately for each drug group.

We judged two studies to have a high risk of incomplete outcome data; Hanlon 1965 and Reardon 1966 had an attrition rate of more than 20% and included only study completers in the final analysis.

Selective reporting

We judged two studies to be free of selective reporting (Hollister 1960; Morton 1968). One study was rated with an unclear risk of selective reporting (Coons 1962). Four studies were rated with a high risk of bias. Of these, the following three studies did not (sufficiently) report on important outcomes: Hanlon 1965, Reardon 1966, Schiele 1961. Leff 1971 did not report results separately for the drug groups.

Other potential sources of bias

We judged four studies to be free of other bias (Hollister 1960; Leff 1971, Reardon 1966; Schiele 1961) and in the remaining three studies this was unclear.

Effects of interventions

See: Summary of findings for the main comparison Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia

For dichotomous data, we calculated risk ratios (RR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI).

1. Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

1.1 Response to treatment

There was no difference in response to treatment as defined by the original studies in three medium‐term studies (trifluoperazine 30%, low‐potency drug 36%, 3 RCTs, n = 120, RR 0.96, CI 0.59 to 1.56).

1.2 Leaving the study early
1.2.1 Acceptability of treatment ‐ Leaving the study early due to any reason

There was no significant difference between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics, neither in the short term (trifluoperazine 25%, low‐potency antipsychotics 21%, 2 RCTs, n = 179, RR 1.23, CI 0.70 to 2.15), nor in the medium term (trifluoperazine 5%, low‐potency antipsychotics 3%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 2.00, CI 0.13 to 30.34), and also not overall (trifluoperazine 20%, low‐potency antipsychotics 16%, 3 RCTs, n = 239, RR 1.25, CI 0.72 to 2.17).

1.2.2 Leaving the study early due to adverse events

There was no significant difference between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics, neither in the short term (trifluoperazine 8%, low‐potency antipsychotics 6%, 2 RCTs, n = 179, RR 1.44. CI 0.48 to 4.33), nor in the medium term (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 0%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR not estimable), and also not overall (trifluoperazine 6%, low‐potency antipsychotics 4%, 3 RCTs, n = 239, RR 1.44, CI 0.48 to 4.33).

1.2.3 Leaving the study early due to inefficacy of treatment

There was no significant difference between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics, neither in the short term (trifluoperazine 3%, low‐potency antipsychotics 1%, 2 RCTs, n = 179, RR 2.00, CI 0.21 to 18.98), nor in the medium term (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 3%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 0.65, CI 0.03 to 15.30), and also not overall (trifluoperazine 2%, low‐potency antipsychotics 1%, 3 RCTs, n = 239, RR 1.37, CI 0.22 to 8.56).

1.3 Relapse

There was no significant difference between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics, neither in the short term (trifluoperazine 9%, low‐potency antipsychotics 9%, 1 RCT, n = 22, RR 1.00, CI 0.07 to 14.05), nor in the medium term (trifluoperazine 15%, low‐potency antipsychotics 6%, 3 RCTs, n = 120, RR 1.64, CI 0.52 to 5.16), and also not in the long term (trifluoperazine 38%, low‐potency antipsychotics 20%, 1 RCT, n = 18, RR 1.92, CI 0.29 to 12.64). The overall results also did not show a significant difference (trifluoperazine 18%, low‐potency antipsychotics 7%, 5 RCTs, n = 160, RR 1.60, CI 0.64 to 4.02).

1.4 Adverse effects
1.4.1 General ‐ At least one adverse effect

There was no significant difference (trifluoperazine 60%, low‐potency antipsychotics 38%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 1.60, CI 0.94 to 2.74).

1.4.2 Specific ‐ Movement disorders

There was a significant difference in terms of at least one movement disorder in favour of low‐potency antipsychotics (trifluoperazine 23%, low‐potency antipsychotics 13%, 2 RCTs, n = 123, RR 2.08, CI 0.78 to 5.55).

a. Akathisia

There was no significant difference (trifluoperazine 6%, low‐potency antipsychotics 1%, 1 RCT, n = 157, RR 6.06, CI 0.65 to 56.82).

b. Akinesia

There was no significant difference (trifluoperazine 2%, low‐potency antipsychotics 1%, 1 RCT, n = 157, RR 2.02, CI 0.13 to 31.64).

c. Dyskinesia

There was no significant difference (trifluoperazine 4%, low‐potency antipsychotics 1%, 1 RCT, n = 157, RR 4.04, CI 0.37 to 43.52).

d. Incoordination

There was a significant difference in favour of low‐potency antipsychotics (trifluoperazine 20%, low‐potency antipsychotics 5%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 7.00, CI 1.60 to 30.66).

e. Rigor

There was a significant difference in favour of low‐potency antipsychotics (trifluoperazine 45%, low‐potency antipsychotics 10%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 4.50, CI 1.58 to 12.84).

1.4.3 Other

a. Anticholinergic

There was no significant difference for blurring of vision (trifluoperazine 5%, low‐potency antipsychotics 0%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 9.76, CI 0.49 to 194.21) or drooling (trifluoperazine 10%, low‐potency antipsychotics 3%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 4.00, CI 0.39 to 41.51).

b. Cardiovascular

There was no significant difference for dizziness (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 1%, 1 RCT, n = 157, RR 0.67, CI 0.03 to 16.09) or syncope (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 3%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 0.65, CI 0.03 to 15.30).

c. Central nervous system

There was no significant difference for a whole series of adverse effects of central nervous system origin including drowsiness (trifluoperazine 20%, low‐potency antipsychotics 23%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 0.89, CI 0.31 to 2.54), insomnia (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 0%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR not estimable), lethargy (trifluoperazine 5%, low‐potency antipsychotics 0%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 5.86, CI 0.25 to 137.66), muscle weakness (trifluoperazine 10%, low‐potency antipsychotics 3%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 4.00, CI 0.39 to 41.51), seizure (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 0%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR not estimable), tenseness (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 3%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 0.65, CI 0.03 to 15.30) and slurred speech (trifluoperazine 5%, low‐potency antipsychotics 3%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 2.00, CI 0.13 to 30.34).

d. Dermatological

There was no significant difference for the outcome of skin disorder (trifluoperazine 4%, low‐potency antipsychotics 5%, 2 RCTs, n = 217, RR 0.99, CI 0.28 to 3.50).

e. Gastrointestinal

There was no significant difference for the outcome of decreased appetite (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 0%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR not estimable).

f. Metabolic

There was no significant difference for polydipsia and polyuria (trifluoperazine 0%, low‐potency antipsychotics 0%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR not estimable).

1.5 Missing outcomes

There were no data on important side‐effects such as hypotension, and sedation, and no data on service use, death and quality of life.

2. Subgroup analyses

All subgroup analyses were conducted only on the primary outcome response to treatment as defined by the original studies.

2.1 Different low‐potency drugs

Three studies compared trifluoperazine with chlorpromazine and there was no significant difference (trifluoperazine 30%, low‐potency drug 33%, 3 RCTs, n = 100, RR 1.05, CI 0.54 to 2.06). The comparison with thioridazine (trifluoperazine 30%, thioridazine 45%, 1 RCT, n = 40, RR 0.89, CI 0.43 to 1.83) also did not lead to a significant difference.

2.2 Clinical state, stage or problem

All three studies with data on response to treatment included participants with chronic schizophrenia. Therefore, this subgroup comparison did not apply.

One study included participants who were treatment resistant and did not show a superiority of trifluoperazine compared with low‐potency antipsychotics (drug 30%, low‐potency antipsychotics 35%, 1 RCT, n = 60, RR 1.14, CI 0.58 to 2.26). There was no significant difference compared to the rest of the studies (test for subgroup differences: Chi² = 0.52, df = 1 (P = 0.47), I² = 0%).

3. Sensitivity analyses

All sensitivity analyses were conducted only on the primary outcome response to treatment as defined by the original studies.

3.1 Exclusion of studies that were judged to be at high risk of bias

There were no studies with data on the primary outcome that were judged to be at high risk of bias.

3.2 Assessment of dosage

There were no clearly discrepant doses between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics.

3.3 Imputed values

This sensitivity analysis did not apply.

3.4 Fixed‐effect model

When a fixed‐effect model was applied, the efficacy of trifluoperazine was also not different from that of low‐potency drugs (trifluoperazine 26%, low‐potency drug 27%, 2 RCTs, n = 60, RR 0.08 CI 0.40 to 1.60).

3.5 Investigation of heterogeneity

There was no heterogeneity for the primary outcome response to treatment (P = 0.61, I2 = 0%).

4. Other results

4.1 Publication bias

Only three studies reported results on the primary outcome response to treatment, therefore we did not use funnel plots.

4.2 'Summary of findings' table

The results of the outcomes response to treatment, leaving the study early, at least one adverse event, at least one movement disorder, sedation, death and quality of life were inspected more closely (see summary of findings Table for the main comparison). Based on this tool, we considered the quality of the results for the outcomes response to treatment and at least one adverse event to be moderate, for leaving the study due to any reason to be low and at least one adverse event to be very low. Moreover, no data on the predefined outcomes death, sedation and quality of life were available. The judgements derived from this instrument were used for the discussion section of the review (see DiscussionSummary of main results).

Discussion

Summary of main results

1. General

Conventional antipsychotic drugs are still used in the treatment of schizophrenia, both in poorer as well as in richer countries. This review compared trifluoperazine with low‐potency conventional antipsychotics, including seven studies with 422 participants. No significant difference between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics was found in the primary outcome response to treatment. This finding is in line with the statements of treatment guidelines that low‐potency drugs are as efficacious as high‐potency antipsychotics such as trifluoperazine and contrasts with a clinical impression that low‐potency conventional antipsychotic drugs are less efficacious. However, there are limitations to this review. Only three studies reported data on the primary outcome. Approximately 1000 participants need to be included in psychiatric meta‐analyses for the results to be robust (Trikalinos 2004). However, the current systematic review included much fewer participants, the primary outcome response to treatment was based on 120 participants, thus it was underpowered. Also, for studies conducted in the 1960s and 1970s operationalised diagnostic criteria such as DSM‐III or its more recent versions were not available. The studies inconsistently addressed and reported the primary outcome clinical response and the methods of sequence generation and blinding were often poorly reported. Other outcomes such as hospital admission, quality of life and employment, which are clinically relevant have not been reported at all. In summary, the included studies do not allow us to make sound conclusions about the relative tolerability and efficacy of trifluoperazine compared with low‐potency antipsychotics. This is also supported by a judgement of low to moderate quality for outcomes in the summary of findings Table for the main comparison.

2. Treatment effects

2.1 Response to treatment

The overall results of response to treatment do not suggest a difference in efficacy between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotic drugs. This result supports early narrative work, which was not based on meta‐analytic methods (Davis 1989; Klein 1969), and does not confirm a clinical perception that low‐potency antipsychotic drugs are less efficacious than trifluoperazine. Due to the fact that only three studies reported on this outcome and that the number of participants was small ‐ altogether 120 participants ‐ this result is inconclusive. Also, the criteria and cut‐offs used for the primary outcome “response to treatment” varied, although a study showed that this is not so much of a problem as long as relative risks and odds ratios are used (Furukawa 2011) .

2.2 Leaving the study early

There was no significant difference between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics for leaving the study early due to any reason; it seems that both trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotics are not different in their overall acceptability for participants with schizophrenia. We also found no significant difference for leaving the study early due to adverse events or inefficacy. However, only three studies reported data on this outcome, which is very little and more data would be needed for a clear interpretation here.

2.3 Adverse effects

For those studies that reported on adverse events, trifluoperazine produced more movement disorders in terms of at least one movement disorder, incoordination and rigor. The findings on extrapyramidal side‐effects are in line with today's knowledge that high‐potency and low‐potency antipsychotics have different affinities and binding properties to dopamine and thus differ in the types of adverse events, such as movement disorders. However, data on other important adverse events such as weight gain and sedation were not reported at all, so differences in affinities to other receptors such as histamine receptors could not be confirmed.

2.4 Other predefined outcomes

None of the included studies reported on cardiac effects, hypotension, sedation, weight gain, service use and death. Data on quality of life, participant´s/carer´s satisfaction with care or economic outcomes were also not reported. These outcomes may be more important for policy makers than conventional measures of efficacy and tolerability. It is therefore disappointing that they are not available.

3. Publication bias

Due to the limited number of studies that reported on the primary outcome, the test for funnel plot asymmetry was not meaningful.

4. Subgroup analyses and investigation of heterogeneity

The effects of trifluoperazine versus each single low‐potency antipsychotic drug did not show any significant difference, but the results were clearly limited by the small number of trials assigned to each single low‐potency drug. There was also no difference in effect between studies with treatment resistant participants and the remaining studies. Other subgroup analyses such as studies with diagnoses other than schizophrenia or first episode were not applicable for the three studies which provided data on the primary outcome response to treatment.

5. Sensitivity analyses

The results of the primary outcome were not different when a fixed‐effects model instead of a random‐effects model was applied. The other predefined sensitivity analyses on risk of bias, dose and imputed values, did not apply to the three studies that reported on the primary outcome.

Overall completeness and applicability of evidence

Most of the included studies were conducted in hospitals and in different countries. Two studies were from the 1970s and all the other studies from the 1960s. Almost all diagnosed participants according to clinical diagnosis as operationalised diagnostic criteria such as DSM‐III or its more recent versions were not available. Thus it is possible that those older studies included participants who nowadays might sometimes have another diagnosis than schizophrenia. Overall, seven of the included studies either did not report on the primary outcome or did not report results independently of the treatment condition. Thus, these included studies could not be used for the main outcome. The evidence on adverse events is particularly incomplete, as none of the included studies reported on important adverse events such as death, suicide, cardiac effects, sedation or weight gain. The comparisons for the primary outcome only included chlorpromazine and one small group that received thioridazine. There were no data on other low‐potency antipsychotics such as mesoridazine, levomepromazine, promazine or perazine, so that the overall evidence on the effectiveness of trifluoperazine compared with low‐potency antipsychotics is incomplete.

New studies with better outcome reporting would be needed in order to make stronger statements about the differences between trifluoperazine and low‐potency antipsychotic medication for routine care.

Quality of the evidence

Almost all studies were randomised and double‐blind but most of them did not describe exact sequence generation, allocation concealment or detailed blinding methods. Lack of blinding is not necessarily a problem for objective outcomes, but problematic for subjective outcomes such as response to treatment or side‐effects. None of the studies were judged to be free of selective reporting, most of them failed to report on previously defined outcomes or did not report the number of participants randomised to each drug group. Most studies did not address the problem of incomplete outcome data, and this might also have influenced the estimates of effect. Therefore, the overall quality of the included studies was low (see summary of findings Table for the main comparison).

Potential biases in the review process

We pooled all low‐potency antipsychotics in one group for all outcomes except for the primary outcome response to treatment, for which we also performed subgroup analyses on trifluoperazine versus each single low‐potency antipsychotic drug. As there were altogether only two different low‐potency antipsychotics and as both of them were from the phenothiazine class, we felt that pooling the results of these antipsychotics was appropriate. Moreover, the search was based on Cochrane Schizophrenia Trials Register and the last search date was 2010, so it is possible that there are unpublished trials that we are not aware of. There is always a possibility of publication bias, but due to the small number of trials this could not be addressed by funnel‐plots. The initiatives to make all studies available to the public are relatively recent, while most studies were old making publication bias likely.

We decided post‐hoc to include all outcomes reported by a study, not only the predefined outcomes. This change to the protocol was made on the basis that other outcomes (e.g. non‐pre‐specified adverse effects) might be important as well and did not affect review authors´ biases.

Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews

We are not aware of other reviews on the efficacy of trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs.

Study flow diagram.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 1

Study flow diagram.

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 2

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.

'Risk of bias' summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.
Figuras y tablas -
Figure 3

'Risk of bias' summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 1 Response to treatment ‐ medium term.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.1

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 1 Response to treatment ‐ medium term.

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 2 Leaving the study early: 1. Acceptability of treatment ‐ leaving early due to any reason.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.2

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 2 Leaving the study early: 1. Acceptability of treatment ‐ leaving early due to any reason.

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 3 Leaving the study early: 2. Due to adverse effects.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.3

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 3 Leaving the study early: 2. Due to adverse effects.

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 4 Leaving the study early: 3. Due to inefficacy.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.4

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 4 Leaving the study early: 3. Due to inefficacy.

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 5 Relapse.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.5

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 5 Relapse.

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 6 Adverse events: 1. General ‐ at least one adverse effect.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.6

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 6 Adverse events: 1. General ‐ at least one adverse effect.

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 7 Adverse events: 2.a. Specific ‐ movement disorders.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.7

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 7 Adverse events: 2.a. Specific ‐ movement disorders.

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 8 Adverse effects: 2.b. Specific ‐ others.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 1.8

Comparison 1 Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS, Outcome 8 Adverse effects: 2.b. Specific ‐ others.

Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis, Outcome 1 Response to treatment ‐ each low‐potency antipsychotic separately.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 2.1

Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis, Outcome 1 Response to treatment ‐ each low‐potency antipsychotic separately.

Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis, Outcome 2 Response to treatment ‐ treatment resistance.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 2.2

Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis, Outcome 2 Response to treatment ‐ treatment resistance.

Comparison 3 Sensitivity analysis, Outcome 1 Response to treatment ‐ fixed effects model.
Figuras y tablas -
Analysis 3.1

Comparison 3 Sensitivity analysis, Outcome 1 Response to treatment ‐ fixed effects model.

Table 2. Other reviews suggested by excluded studies

Suggested title

Excluded study tag

Alternate use of different antipsychotics for schizophrenia

Wilson 1961 (trifluoperazine and chlorpromazine)

Combined multi‐drug therapy for schizophrenia

Casey 1961

Trifluoperazine and chlorpromazine combination for schizophrenia

DeWolfe 1971, Smith 1958

Trifluoperazine doses for schizophrenia

Crane 1970, Prien 1969

Trifluoperazine versus mid‐potency antipsychotics for schizophrenia

Gardos 1970 (thiothixene), Hamilton 1963 (prochlorperazine)

Trifluoperazine versus other high‐potency antipsychotics and anxiolytics for schizophrenia

Marjerrison 1966 (triperidol, haloanisone), Prien 1968

Trifluoperazine versus other phenothiazine (unknown) antipsychotics for schizophrenia

Rudy 1958

Trifluoperazine versus placebo

Hershon 1972, Platz 1967, Prien 1969, Weston 1961

Figuras y tablas -
Table 2. Other reviews suggested by excluded studies
Table 3. Design of a future study

Methods

Allocation: randomised ‐ clearly described generation of sequence and concealment of allocation.
Blinding: double ‐ described and tested.
Duration: long term.

Participants

People with schizophrenia or schizophrenia‐like disorder.
N = 500.
Age: any.
Sex: both.
History: any.

Interventions

1. Trifluoperazine (oral).

2. Any low‐potency antipsychotic (oral).

Outcomes

Response (primary outcome)

Rehospitalisation

Mental state (BPRS)

Global state (CGI)

Leaving the study early (including specific causes)

Death (natural and unnatural causes)

Side‐effects

Quality of life

Satisfaction with care

Employment

BPRS ‐ Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale
CGI ‐ Clinical Global Impression

Figuras y tablas -
Table 3. Design of a future study
Summary of findings for the main comparison. Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia

Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia

Patient or population: patients with schizophrenia
Settings: Inpatients and outpatients
Intervention: Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of Participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Control

Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Response to treatment ‐ medium term
Follow‐up: 4‐6 months

Study population

RR 0.96
(0.59 to 1.56)

120
(3 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
low1,2

364 per 1000

349 per 1000
(215 to 567)

Moderate

350 per 1000

336 per 1000
(206 to 546)

Leaving the study early: 1. Acceptability of treatment ‐ leaving early due to any reason
Follow‐up: 1‐4 months

Study population

RR 1.25
(0.72 to 2.17)

239
(3 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
low1,2

160 per 1000

200 per 1000
(115 to 348)

Moderate

182 per 1000

228 per 1000
(131 to 395)

Adverse events: 1. General ‐ at least one adverse effect
Follow‐up: mean 4 months

Study population

RR 1.6
(0.94 to 2.74)

60
(1 study)

⊕⊕⊕⊝
moderate1

375 per 1000

600 per 1000
(352 to 1000)

Moderate

375 per 1000

600 per 1000
(352 to 1000)

Adverse events: 2.a. Specific ‐ movement disorders ‐ at least one movement disorder
Follow‐up: 6‐26 weeks

Study population

RR 2.08
(0.78 to 5.55)

123
(2 studies)

⊕⊕⊝⊝
low1,3

89 per 1000

186 per 1000
(70 to 496)

Moderate

50 per 1000

104 per 1000
(39 to 278)

Death

See comment

See comment

Not estimable

0
(04)

See comment

Sedation

See comment

See comment

Not estimable

0
(05)

See comment

Quality of life

See comment

See comment

Not estimable

0
(05)

See comment

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1 Risk of bias: rated 'serious' ‐ most information is from studies at unclear risk of bias, missing or unclear results for incomplete outcome data and selective reporting
2 Imprecision: rated 'serious' ‐ the total number of events is less than 300 and the estimate of effect includes appreciable benefit/harm.
3 Imprecision: rated 'serious' ‐ the total number of events is less than 300 and the CI is quite wide
4 There were no data for this very important outcome
5 There were no data for this outcome

Figuras y tablas -
Summary of findings for the main comparison. Trifluoperazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia
Table 1. Series of similar reviews

Title

Reference

Haloperidol versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Tardy 2011b

Flupenthixol versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Tardy 2011

Fluphenazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Tardy 2011a

Perphenazine versus low‐potency antipsychotic drugs

Tardy 2011c

Figuras y tablas -
Table 1. Series of similar reviews
Comparison 1. Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Response to treatment ‐ medium term Show forest plot

3

120

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.96 [0.59, 1.56]

2 Leaving the study early: 1. Acceptability of treatment ‐ leaving early due to any reason Show forest plot

3

239

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.25 [0.72, 2.17]

2.1 short term

2

179

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.23 [0.70, 2.15]

2.2 medium term

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

2.0 [0.13, 30.34]

3 Leaving the study early: 2. Due to adverse effects Show forest plot

3

239

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.44 [0.48, 4.33]

3.1 short term

2

179

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.44 [0.48, 4.33]

3.2 medium term

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

4 Leaving the study early: 3. Due to inefficacy Show forest plot

3

239

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.37 [0.22, 8.56]

4.1 short term

2

179

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

2.0 [0.21, 18.98]

4.2 medium term

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.65 [0.03, 15.30]

5 Relapse Show forest plot

5

160

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.60 [0.64, 4.02]

5.1 short term

1

22

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.0 [0.07, 14.05]

5.2 medium term

3

120

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.64 [0.52, 5.16]

5.3 long term

1

18

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.92 [0.29, 12.64]

6 Adverse events: 1. General ‐ at least one adverse effect Show forest plot

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.6 [0.94, 2.74]

7 Adverse events: 2.a. Specific ‐ movement disorders Show forest plot

4

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

7.1 at least one movement disorder

2

123

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

2.08 [0.78, 5.55]

7.2 akathisia

1

157

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

6.06 [0.65, 56.82]

7.3 akinesia

1

157

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

2.02 [0.13, 31.64]

7.4 dyskinesia

1

157

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

4.04 [0.37, 43.52]

7.5 incoordination

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

7.0 [1.60, 30.66]

7.6 rigor

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

4.5 [1.58, 12.84]

8 Adverse effects: 2.b. Specific ‐ others Show forest plot

2

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

8.1 anticholinergic ‐ blurred vision

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

9.76 [0.49, 194.21]

8.2 anticholinergic ‐ drooling

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

4.0 [0.39, 41.51]

8.3 cardiovascular ‐ dizziness

1

157

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.67 [0.03, 16.09]

8.4 cardiovascular ‐ syncope

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.65 [0.03, 15.30]

8.5 central nervous system ‐ drowsiness

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.89 [0.31, 2.54]

8.6 central nervous system ‐ insomnia

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

8.7 central nervous system ‐ lethargy

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

5.86 [0.25, 137.66]

8.8 central nervous system ‐ muscle weakness

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

4.0 [0.39, 41.51]

8.9 central nervous system ‐ seizure

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

8.10 central nervous system ‐ slurred speech

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

2.0 [0.13, 30.34]

8.11 central nervous system ‐ tenseness

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.65 [0.03, 15.30]

8.12 dermatological ‐ skin disorder

2

217

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.99 [0.28, 3.50]

8.13 gastrointestinal ‐ decreased appetite

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

8.14 metabolic ‐ polydipsia and polyuria

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.0 [0.0, 0.0]

Figuras y tablas -
Comparison 1. Comparison 1: TRIFLUOPERAZINE versus LOW‐POTENCY ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS
Comparison 2. Subgroup analysis

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Response to treatment ‐ each low‐potency antipsychotic separately Show forest plot

3

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

1.1 versus chlorpromazine

3

100

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.05 [0.54, 2.06]

1.2 versus thioridazine

1

40

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.89 [0.43, 1.83]

2 Response to treatment ‐ treatment resistance Show forest plot

3

120

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.96 [0.59, 1.56]

2.1 Not treatment resistant

2

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

0.8 [0.40, 1.60]

2.2 Treatment resistant

1

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI)

1.14 [0.58, 2.26]

Figuras y tablas -
Comparison 2. Subgroup analysis
Comparison 3. Sensitivity analysis

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Response to treatment ‐ fixed effects model Show forest plot

2

60

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.8 [0.40, 1.60]

Figuras y tablas -
Comparison 3. Sensitivity analysis